Skip to main content
Log in

Cognitive attribution of the source of an error in object-lifting results in differences in motor generalization

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To lift an object, the motor system must predict the weight of the object and use this information to program appropriate lifting forces. If this prediction is erroneous, people may assign blame for the error to either themselves or an external source—a process called credit assignment. In the present study, we explored the role of credit assignment on weight predictions during a lifting task. Participants were told that the EMG surface electrodes attached to their lifting hand were either part of a “passive” system that recorded muscular activity, or part of an “active” system that would apply energy to the muscle, influencing weight perception. Participants performed 90 lifts of the training blocks, followed by 10 lifts of a newly encountered larger test block. In between training and test trials, the experimenter turned off the recording system and removed the surface electrodes for participants in the “active” group. For each lift, we determined the initial peak rate of change of vertical load force rate and load-phase duration, estimates of predicted object weight. Analysis of the first 10 training lifts and the last 10 training lifts revealed no effect of Active versus Passive EMG on weight predictions. However, after removing the EMG equipment, participants in the “active” group failed to scale their predictive load forces in the same manner as those in the “passive” condition when lifting a novel block. We conclude that cognitive information may play a role in credit assignment, influencing weight prediction when lifting novel objects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baugh LA, Kao M, Johansson RS, Flanagan JR (2012) Material evidence: interaction of well-learned priors and sensorimotor memory when lifting objects. J Neurophysiol 108:1262–1269

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brodie EE, Ross HE (1984) Sensorimotor mechanisms in weight discrimination. Percept Psychophys 36:477–481

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G (2014) Getting a grip on heaviness perception: a review of weight illusions and their probable causes. Exp Brain Res 232:1623–1629

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G, Goodale MA (2010) Lifting without seeing: the role of vision in perceiving and acting upon the size weight illusion. PLoS ONE 5:e9709

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G, Cant JS, Goodale MA (2009) Living in a material world: how visual cues to material properties affect the way that we lift objects and perceive their weight. J Neruophysiol 102:3111–3118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckingham G, Wong JD, Tang M, Gribble PL, Goodale MA (2014) Observing object lifting errors modulates cortico-spinal excitability and improves object lifting performance. Cortex 50:115–124

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Donchin O, Gazzaniga MS, Shadmehr R (2003) Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. J Neurophysiol 89:168–176

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Bastian AJ, Shadmehr R (2010) Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. J Neurophysiol 103:2275–2284

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Diedrichsen J, White O, Newman D, Lally N (2010) Use-dependent and error-based learning of motor behaviors. J Neurosci 30:5159–5166

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fercho K, Baugh LA (2014) It’s too quick to blame myself-the effects of fast and slow rates of change on credit assignment during object lifting. Front Hum Neurosci 8:554

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Beltzner MA (2000) Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size-weight illusion. Nat Neurosci 3:737–741

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1997) The role of internal models in motion planning and control: evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. J Neurosci 17:1519–1528

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Bowman MC, Johansson RS (2006) Control strategies in object manipulation tasks. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:650–659

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Bittner JP, Johansson RS (2008) Experience can change distinct size-weight priors engaged in lifting objects and judging their weights. Curr Biol 18:1742–1747

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Johansson RS, Westling G (1991) Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: comments on the contribution of size cues. Exp Brain Res 85:226–229

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon AM, Westling G, Cole K, Johansson RS (1993) Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J Neurophysiol 69:1789–1796

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hatada Y, Miall R, Rossetti Y (2006) Two waves of a long-lasting aftereffect of prism adaptation measured over 7 days. Exp Brain Res 169:417–426

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huang VS, Shadmehr R (2009) Persistence of motor memories reflects statistics of the learning event. J Neurophysiol 102:931–940

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Huang VS, Haith A, Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW (2011) Rethinking motor learning and savings in adaptation paradigms: model-free memory for successful actions combines with internal models. Neuron 70:787–801

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hwang EJ, Smith MA, Shadmehr R (2006) Dissociable effects of the implicit and explicit memory systems on learning control of reaching. Exp Brain Res 173:425–437

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson RS, Cole K (1992) Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2:815–823

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson RS, Westling G (1988) Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneuously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip. Exp Brain Res 71:59–71

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones LA (1986) Perception of force and weight: theory and research. Psychol Bull 100:29–42

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Stelmach GE (1997) Adaptation to gradual as compared with sudden visuo-motor distortions. Exp Brain Res 115:557–561

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klassen J, Tong C, Flanagan JR (2005) Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations. Exp Brain Res 164:250–259

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kluzik J, Diedrichsen J, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ (2008) Reach adaptation: what determines whether we learn an internal model of the tool or adapt the model of our arm? J Neurophysiol 100:1455–1464

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer JW, Ghez C, Ghilardi MF (2005) Adaptation to visuomotor transformations: consolidation, interference, and forgetting. J Neurosci 25:473–478

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, DiZio P (2005) Motor control and learning in altered dynamic environments. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:653–659

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malfait N, Ostry DJ (2004) Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the sudden introduction of load? J Neurosci 24:8084–8089. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-04.2004

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miall RC, Wolpert DM (1996) Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw 9:1265–1279

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michel C, Pisella L, Prablanc C, Rode G, Rossetti Y (2007) Enhancing visuomotor adaptation by reducing error signals: single-step (aware) versus multiple-step (unaware) exposure to wedge prisms. J Cogn Neurosci 19:341–350

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mon-Williams M, Murray AH (2000) The size of the visual size cue used for programming manipulative forces during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 135:405–410

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newell KM (1991) Motor skill acquisition. Annu Rev Psychol 42:213–237

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pang X, Tan HZ, Durlach N (1991) Manual discrimination of force using active finger motion. Percept Psychophys 49:531–540

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelisson D, Alahyane N, Panouilleres M, Tilikete C (2010) Sensorimotor adaptation of saccadic eye movements. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:1103–1120

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redding GM, Wallace B (1996) Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:379–394

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1994) Adaptive representation of specific dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci 14:3208–3224

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R (2006) Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4:e179

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thoroughman KA, Shadmehr R (2000) Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature 407:742–747

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwislocki JJ, Goodman DA (1980) Absolute scaling of sensory magnitudes: a validation. Percept Psyophys 28:28–38

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lee A. Baugh.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fercho, K., Baugh, L.A. Cognitive attribution of the source of an error in object-lifting results in differences in motor generalization. Exp Brain Res 234, 2667–2676 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4670-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4670-0

Keywords

Navigation