Skip to main content
Log in

Nonabsolute/relativistic thinking: A common factor underlying models of postformal reasoning?

  • Published:
Journal of Adult Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Strong empirical evidence was obtained in support of the proposition that nonabsolute/relativistic (N/R) thinking is a unifying commonality underlying four postformal models of higher-order thinking, namely, problem finding, dialectical reasoning, relativistic operations, and reflective judgment. Within the construct of N/R thinking, two dimensions—the basic form and the epistemic view—and two levels—the formal form and the postformal form—were differentiated as hypothesized. The findings clarify logical relationships among the models as well as provide a common link unifying their diversity. Implications and suggestions for future research are provided.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arlin, P. K. (1974).Problem finding: The relation between selected cognitive process variables and problem-finding performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.

  • Arlin, P. K. (1975). Cognitive development in adulthood: A fifth stage?Developmental Psychology, 11, 602–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1975/76). A cognitive process model of problem finding.Educational Horizons, 54, 99–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1982). A multi-trait multi-method validity study of a test of formal reasoning.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 43, 103–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1984a). Adolescent and adult thought: A structural interpretation. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.),Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 258–271). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1984b).The Arlin Test of Formal Reasoning. New York: Slosson Educational Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1986). Problem finding and young adult cognition. In R. A. Mines & K. Kitchener (Eds.),Adult cognitive development: Methods and models (pp. 22–32). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1989). Problem solving and problem finding in young artists and young scientists. In M. L. Commons, J. D. Sinnott, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.),Adult development Vol. 1: Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 197–216). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K. (1990). Wisdom: The art of problem finding. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 230–243). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arlin, P. K., & Fung L. (1995, April).Wisdom and expertise in teaching: Searching for developmental indicators. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.

  • Basseches, M. (1980). Dialectical schemata: A framework for the empirical study of the development of dialectical thinking.Human Development, 23, 400–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benack, S., & Basseches, M. (1989). Dialectical thinking and relativistic epistemology: Their relation in adult development. In M. L. Commons, J. D. Sinnott, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.),Adult development, Vol. 1: Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 95–112). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1978). The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 173–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M. (1984). Not beyond formal operations but beyond Piaget. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.),Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 395–412). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanaugh, J. C., Kramer, D. A., Sinnott, J. D., Camp, C. J., & Markley, R. P. (1985). On missing links and such: Interfaces between cognitive research and everyday problem-solving.Human Development, 28, 146–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanaugh, J. C., & Stafford, H. (1989). Being aware of issues and biases: Directions for research on postformal thought. In M. L. Commoms, J. D. Sinnott, F. A. Richard, & C. Armon (Eds.),Adult development, Vol. 1: Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 279–292). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Richards, F. A., & Kuhn, D. (1982). Systematic and metasystematic reasoning: A case for a level of reasoning beyond Piaget's formal operations.Child Development, 53, 1058–1069.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Richards, F. A., & Armon, C. (Eds.). (1984).Beyond formal operations: Late adolescence and adult cognitive development. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Sinnott, J. D., Richards, F. A., & Armon, C. (Eds.). (1989).Adult development, Vol. 1: Comparisons and applications of developmental models. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Commons, M. L., Armon, C., Kohlberg, L., Richards, F. A., Grotzer, T. A., & Sinnott, J. D. (Eds.), (1990).Adult development, Vol. 2: Models and methods in the study of adolescent and adult thought. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cudeck, R. (1989). Analysis of correlation matrices using covariance structure models.Psychological Bulletin, 105, 317–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W. (1964). Creative thinking, problem solving and instruction. In E. Hilgard (Ed.),The sixty-third year book of the National Society for the Study of Education: Theories of learning and instruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1970). Concern for discovery: An attitudinal component of creative production.Journal of Personality, 38, 91–105.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H. E. (1973). Courage and cognitive growth in children and scientists. In M. Schwebel & J. Ralph (Eds.),Piaget in the classroom. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of the intellect.Psychological Bulletin, 53, 267–293.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Infeld, L., & Einstein, A. (1938).The evolution of physics: The growth of ideas from early concepts to relativity and quanta. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958).Growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1993).LISREL 8: User's reference guide. Chicago: Scientific Software International.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, P. M., Kitchener, K. S., Davison, M. L., Parker, C., & Wood, P. K. (1983). The justification of beliefs in young adults: A longitudinal study.Human Development, 26, 106–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S. (1983). Cognition, metacognition, and epistemic cognition: A three-level model of cognitive processing.Human Development, 26, 222–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S. (1986). The reflective judgment model: Characteristics, evidence, and measurement. In R. A. Mines & K. S. Kitchener (Eds.),Adult cognitive development: Methods and models (pp. 76–91). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., & Brenner, H. G. (1990). Wisdom and reflective judgment: Knowing in the face of uncertainty. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Wisdom: Its nature, origins and development (pp. 212–229). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., & King, P. M. (1981). Reflective Judgement: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitchener, K. S., Lynch, C. L., Fischer, K. W., & Wood, P. K. (1993). Developmental range of reflective judgment: The effect of contextual support and practice on developmental stage.Developmental Psychology, 29, 893–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, D. (1983). Post-formal operations? A need for further conceptualization.Human Development, 26, 91–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labouvie-Vief, G., Adams, C., Hakim-Larson, J., & Hayden, M. (1983, April).Contexts of logic: The growth of interpretation from pre-adolescence to mature adulthood. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit.

  • Mackworth, N. H. (1965). Originality.American Psychologist, 20, 51–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mines, R. A., & Kitchener, K. S. (Eds.). (1986).Adult cognitive development: methods and models. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riegel, K. (1973). Dialectic operations: The final period of cognitive development.Human Development, 16, 346–370.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1981). Developmental sequences within and between concepts.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46, (2, Serial No. 189).

  • Sinnott, J. D. (1981). The theory of relativity: A metatheory for Development?Human Development, 24, 293–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinnott, J. D. (1984). Post formal reasoning: The relativistic stage. In M. L. Commons, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.),Beyond formal operations: Late adolescent and adult cognitive development (pp. 294–325). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinnott, J. D. (1989). Life-span relativistic postformal thought: Methodology and data from everyday problem-solving studies. In M. L. Commons, J. D. Sinnott, F. A. Richards, & C. Armon (Eds.),Adult development, Vol. 1: Comparisons and applications of developmental models (pp. 239–278). New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinnott, J. D. (1994). Postformal thought, adult learning, and lifespan development: How are they related? In J. Sinnott (Ed.),Interdisciplinary handbook of adult lifespan learning. Connecticut: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smilansky, J. (1985). Problem finding and creative problem solving.Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 377–384.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1989).Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I. A. (1972).A theory of creative transect-ualization. Greensboro, NC: Center for Creative Leadership.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, B. (1995).Nonabsolute/relativistic (N/R) thinking: A possible unifying commonality underlying models of postformal reasoning. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, B., & Arlin, P. K. (1993a).The Test of the Postformal Form of Nonabsolute/Relativistic (N/R) Thinking (N/R-PF). Unpublished test, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yan, B., & Arlin, P. K. (1993b).The Test of the Epistemic View of Nonabsolute/Relativistic (N/R) Thinking (N/R-EV). Unpublished test, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Yan, B., Arlin, P.K. Nonabsolute/relativistic thinking: A common factor underlying models of postformal reasoning?. J Adult Dev 2, 223–240 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02251038

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02251038

Key words

Navigation