Skip to main content
Log in

Parallel distributed processing and neuropsychology: A neural network model of Wisconsin card sorting and verbal fluency

  • Published:
Neuropsychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Neural networks can be used as a tool in the explanation of neuropsychological data. Using the Hebbian Learning Rule and other such principles as competition and modifiable interlevel feedback, researchers have successfully modeled a widely used neuropsychological test, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. One of these models is reviewed here and extended to a qualitative analysis of how verbal fluency might be modeled, which demonstrates the importance of accounting for the attentional components of both tests. Difficulties remain in programming sequential cognitive processes within a parallel distributed processing (PDP) framework and integrating exceedingly complex neuropsychological tests such as Proverbs. PDP neural network methodology offers neuropsychologists co-validation procedures within narrowly defined areas of reliability and validity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. A., and Rosenfeld, E. (1988).Neurocomputing MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. A., Pellionisz, A., and Rosenfeld, E. (1990).Neurocomputing 2 MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A., and Robinson, J. A. (1990).Natural and Artificial Parallel Computation MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, D. W. (1986). Cortical connections and parallel processing: Structure and function.The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9: 67–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benton, A. L., and Hamsher, K. (1989).Multilingual Aphasia Examination (2nd ed.) University of Iowa and AJA Associates, Iowa City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, E. D. (1988). Frontal lobe damage and neuropsychological assessment.Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 3: 115–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliss, T. V. P., and Lomo, T. (1973). Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path.Journal of Physiology (London) 232: 331–356.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandt, J. (1990). D2 receptors in Huntington's disease: Positron emission tomography findings and clinical correlates.Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 2: 20–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brechtereva, N. P., Abdulldev, Y. G., and Medvedev, S. V. (1991). Neuronal activity in frontal speech area 44 of the human cortex during word recognition.Neuroscience Letters 124: 61–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985).Conceptual Change in Childhood MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, G. A., and Grossberg, S. (1987). A massively parallel architecture for a self-organizing neural pattern recognition machine.Computer Vision, Graphics, and Image Processing 37: 54–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, G. A., and Grossberg, S. (1990). ART 3: Hierarchical search using chemical transmitters in self-organizing pattern recognition architectures.Neural Networks 3: 129–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caudill, M., and Butler, C. (1990).Naturally Intelligent Systems MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chater, N., and Oaksford, M. (1990). Autonomy implementation and cognitive architecture: A reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn.Cognition 34: 93–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., and Servan-Schreiber, D. (1989). A parallel distributed processing approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia.Technical Report AIP-100. Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D. and Servan-Schreiberg, D., (1992). Introduction of Neural Networks Models in psychiatry.Psychiatric Annals 22, 113–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. A., and Grossberg, S. (1987). A massively-parallel architecture for learning, recognizing, and predicting multiple groupings of patterned data.Applied Optics 26: 1866–1891.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., and McClelland, J. L. (1990). A parallel distributed processing model of the Stroop effect.Psychological Review 97: 332–361.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, L. (1988). Clinical neuropsychology: Prospects and problems.The Clinical Neuropsychologist 2: 3–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockett, D., Bilsker, D., Hurwitz, T., and Kozak, J. (1986). Clinical utility of three measures of frontal lobe dysfunction in neuropsychiatric samples.International Journal of Neuroscience 30: 241–248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1990). Category-related recognition defected as a clue to the neural substrates of knowledge.Trends in Neurosciences 13: 95–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., and Damasion, H. (1990). Face agnosia and the neural substrates of memory.Annual Review of Neuroscience 13: 89–109.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., and Changeux, J.-P. (1989). A simple model of prefrontal cortex function in delayed-response tasks.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1: 244–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., and Changeux, J.-P. (1991). The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Theoretical analysis and modeling in a neuronal network.Cerebral Cortex 1: 62–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M., and Mountcastle, V. (1978).The Mindful Brain MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. and McClelland, J. (1992). Neural Network Models and cognitive neuropsychology.Psychiatric Annals 22, 148–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, J. A., and Ballard, D. H. (1982). Connectionist models and their properties.Cognitive Science 6: 205–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A., and Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis.Cognition 28: 3–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foote, S. W., and Morrison, J. K. (1987). Extrathalamic cortical modulation.Annual Review of Neuroscience 10: 67–95.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. (1989).The Prefrontal Cortex 2nd ed., Raven, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children.Cognition 23: 189–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, A. (1990). New maps of the human brain.Science 249: 122–123.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Globus, G. G. (1992). Toward a noncomputational cognitive neuroscience.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4: 299–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden. R. M. (1986). A developmental neural model of visual word perception.Cognitive Science 10: 241–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorham, D. R. (1956). A proverbs test for clinical and experimental use.Psychological Reports 2: 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorham, D. R. (1975).Clinical Manual for the Proverbs Test Psychological Test Specialists, Missoula. MT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graubard, S. R. (1988).The Artificial Intelligence Debate MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1972a). A neural theory of punishment and avoidance. I. Quantitative theory.Mathematical Biosciences 15: 39–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1972b). A neural theory of punishment and avoidance. II. Quantitative theory.Mathematical Biosciences 15: 253–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1987). Competitive learning: From interactive activation to adaptive resonance.Cognitive Science 11: 23–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S. (1988).Neural Networks and Natural Intelligence MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S., and Levine, D. S. (1975). Some developmental and attentional biases in the contrast enhancement and short term memory of recurrent neural networks.Journal of Theoretical Biology 53: 341–380.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S., and Mingolla, E. (1985). Neural dynamics of form perception: Boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading.Psychology Review 92: 173–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, S. J., and Kegl, J. (1987). PARSNIP: A connectionist network that learns natural language grammar from exposure to natural language sentences.Proceedings of the Ninth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949).The Organization of Behavior Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hestenes, D. (1991). A neural network theory of manic-depressive illness. In Levine, D. S. and Leven, S. J. (eds.),Motivation, Emotion, and Goal Direction in Neural Networks Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 209–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. E., McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1986). Distributed representations. In Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. L. (eds.),Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition. Vol. 1, Foundations MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 77–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kandel, E. R., and Tauc, L. (1965). Heterosynaptic facilitation in neurons of the abdominal ganglion ofAplysia depilans.Journal of Physiology (London) 181: 1–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1986). The acquisition of natural kinds of artifact terms. In Demopolous, W., and Marras, A. (eds.),Language Learning and Concept Acquisition: Foundational Issues. Ablex, Norwood, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopf, A. H. (1988). A neuronal model of classical conditioning.Psychobiology 16: 85–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C., and Segev, I. (1989).Methods in Neural Modeling. MIT Press. Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolb, B., and Whishaw, I. Q. (1990).Fundamentals of human neuropsychology 3rd ed., W. H. Freeman, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosko, B. (1986). Differential Hebbian learning. In Denker, J. S. (ed.),Neural Networks for Computing, AIP Conference Proceedings, Vol. 151, American Institute of Physics, New York, pp. 277–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachter, J., and Bever, T. G. (1988). The relation between linguistic structure and associative theories of language learning- A constructive critique of some connectionist learning models.Cognition 28: 195–247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leven, S. J., and Levine, D. S. (1987). Effects of reinforcement of knowledge retrieval and evaluation.Proceedings of the First International Conference on Neural Works, Vol. 2, IEEE/ICNN, San Diego, CA, pp. 269–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, S., Yurgelun-Todd, D., and Craft, S. (1989). Contributions of clinical neuropsychology to the study of schizophrenia.Journal of Abnormal Psychology 98: 341–356.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S. (1983). Neural population modeling and psychology: A review.Mathematical Biosciences 66: 1–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S. (1989a). Neural network principles for theoretical psychology.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 21: 213–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S. (1989b, December). The third wave in neural networks.AI Expert pp. 26–33.

  • Levine, D. S. (1991).Introduction to Neural and Cognitive Modeling Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S., and Bapi, R. S. (1990). Networks modeling the involvement of the frontal lobes in learning and performance of flexible movement sequences.IJCNN International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, San Diego, California, Vol. 2, pp. 759–764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S., and Leven, S. J. (1991).Motivation, Emotion, and Goal Direction in Neural Networks Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S., and Prueitt, P. S. (1989). Modeling some effects of frontal lobe damage: Novelty and perseveration.Neural Networks 2: 103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D. S., Leven, S. J., and Prueitt, P. S. (1991). Integration, disintegration, and the frontal lobes. In Levine, D. S., and Leven, S. J. (eds.),Motivation, Emotion, and Goal Direction in Neural Networks Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 301–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lezak, M. (1983).Neuropsychological assessment 2nd ed., Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, D. L., Graesser, A. C., and Long, C. J. (1988). Four computational models for investigating neuropsychological decision-making. In Williams, J. M., and Long, C. J. (eds.),Cognitive Approaches to Neuropsychology Plenum, New York, pp. 3–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982).Vision. Freeman, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W. (1986, November). Connectionist models of the mind. Paper presented at theTenth Psychonomic Society Meeting. New Orleans, LA.

  • Massaro, D. W. (1988). Some criticisms of connectionist models of human performance.Journal of Memory and Language 27: 213–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. E., and Rumelhart, D. I. (1981). An interactive model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings.Psychological Review 88: 375–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., and Cohen, J. J. (1989). Catastrophic interference in connectionist networks: The sequential learning problem. In Bower, G. H. (ed.),The Psychology of Learning and Motivation Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGeer, E. G., Peppard, R. P., McGeer, P. L., Tuokko, H., Crockett, D., Parks, R., Akiyama, H., Calne, D. B., Beattie, B. L., and Harrop, R. (1990). Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography studies in presumed Alzheimer cases, including 13 serial scans.Canadian Journal of Neurological Science 17: 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In Warren, J., and Akert, K. (eds.),The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior, McGraw-Hill, New York, pp. 313–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishkin, M., and Appenzeller, T. (1987, June). The anatomy of memory.Scientific American, pp. 80–89.

  • Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., and Bachevalier, J. (1984). Memories and habits: Two neural systems. In Lynch, G., McGaugh, J., and Weinberger, N. (eds.),Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Guilford, New York, pp. 65–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, G., and Murphy, L. B. (1969).Western Psychology Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nauta, W. J. H. (1971). The problem of the frontal lobe: A reinterpretation.Journal of Psychiatric Research 8: 167–187.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, F. (1969).Missile Wounds of the Brain Oxford University Press, London. Neural Network Study. (1988)DARPA, AFCEA International Press, Fairfax, VA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1986). Reflections on cognition and parallel distributed processing. In McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (eds.),Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Volume 2: Psychological and Biological Models. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 531–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R.W., Crockett, D. J., and McGeer, P. L. (1989). Systems interpretation of cortical organization: Positron emission tomography and neuropsychological test performance. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 4: 335–349.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R. W., Loewenstein, D. A., and Chang, J. Y. (1988). Brain imaging: Positron emission tomography and cognitive functioning. In Williams, J. M., and Long, C. J. (eds.),Cognitive Approaches to Neuropsychology Plenum, New York, pp. 189–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R. W, Loewenstein, D. A., Dodrill, K. L., Barker, W. W., Yoshii, F., Change, J. Y., Emran, A., Apicella, A., Sheramata, W. A., and Duara, R. (1988). Cerebral metabolic effects of a verbal fluency test: A PET scan study.Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 10: 565–575.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R. W., Crockett, D. J., Tuokko, H., Beattie, B L., Ashford, J. W., Coburn, K. L., Zec, R. F., Becker, R. E., McGeer, P. L., and McGeer, E. G. (1989). Neuropsychological “systems efficiency” and positron emission tomography.Journal of Neuropsychiatry 1: 269–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R. W., Becker, R. E., Dodrill, K. L., Bennett, B. A., Crockett, D. J., Hurwitz, T. A., McGeer, P. L., and McGeer, E. G. (1990). Positron emission tomography and neuropsychological studies in dementia. In Becker, R. E., and Giacobini, E. (eds.),Alzheimer Therapy: Early Diagnosis Taylor & Francis, New York, pp. 315–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R. W., Long, D. L., Levine, D. A., Crockett, D. J., Dalton, I. E., Zec, F. R., Coburn, K. L., Siler, G., Nelson, M. E., Bower, J. M., Becker, R. E., McGeer, E. G., and McGeer, P. L. (1991). Parallel distributed processing and neural networks: Origins, methodology and cognitive functions.International Journal of Neuroscience 60: 195–214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks, R. W., Coburn, K. L., Zec, R. F., Ashford, J. W., Crockett, D. J., Kuhn, M., Moreno, M. A., Rashid, A., Vicari, S., and Feldman, E. (1992). Electrocortical mapping, MRI, and neuropsychological measures: Evidence of Alzheimer's disease in the presence of vascular lesions.Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 6: 393–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavel, M., Gluck, M. A., and Henkle, V. (1988, August). Generalization by human and multilayer adaptive networks.Tenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Montreal, Canada.

  • Pawlik, G., and Heiss, W. D. (1989). Positron emission tomography and neuropsychological function. In Bigler, E. E., Yeo, R. A., and Turkheimer, E. (eds.),Neuropsychological Function and Brain Imaging Plenum Press, New York, pp. 65–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pazzani, M. (1987). Explanation based learning for knowledge based systems.International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 26: 413–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perret, W. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior.Neuropsychologia 12: 323–330.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., and Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.Cognition 28: 73–193.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinto-Hamuy, T., and Linck, P. (1965). Effects of frontal lesions on performance of sequential tasks by monkeys.Experimental Neurology 12: 96–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pool, R. (1992). Computing in science.Science 256, 44–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K. H. (1961). A further experimental analysis of the behavioral deficit that follows injury to the primate frontal cortex.Experimental Neurology 3: 432–466.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rand, B. (1924).Modern Classical Philosophers 2nd ed., Houghton Mifflin, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1990). Connectionist models of recognition memory: Constraints imposed by learning and forgetting functions.Psychological Review 97: 285–308.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ricart, R. (1991). Neuromodulatory mechanisms in neural networks and their influence on interstimulus interval effects in Pavlovian conditioning. In Levine, D. S., and Leven, S. J. (eds.),Motivation, Emotion, and Goal Direction in Neural Networks Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp. 117–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, L. (1989). Are neural nets like the human brain?Science 243: 481–482.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., and McClelland, J. E. (1986).Parallel Distributed Processing, Vol. I and II MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. and Zipser, D. (1985). Feature discovery by competitive learning.Cognitive Science 9: 75–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1987). Connectionism: Is it a paradigm shift for psychology?Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 19: 73–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siler, W., and Tucker, D. (1990). Patterns of inductive reasoning in a parallel expert system. In Gains, B. R., and Boose, J. H. (eds.),Machine Learning and Uncertain Reasoning, Academic Press, London, England, pp. 93–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism.The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 1–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. L., and Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect.Psychological Review 81: 119–145.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., and Benson, D. F. (1983). Frontal lobe lesions and behavior. In Kertesz, A. (ed.),Localization in Neuropsychology Academic Press, New York, pp. 429–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., and Benson, D. F. (1986).The Frontal Lobes Raven, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1981).Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised The Psychological Corporation, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, A. J., Shallice, T., and McCarthy, R. (1987). Frontal lesions and sustained attention.Neuropsychologia 25: 359–365.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witten, I. H., and MacDonald, B. A (1990). Using concept learning for knowledge acquisition. In Gaines, B. R., and Boose, J. H. (eds.),Machine Learning and Uncertain Reasoning Academic Press, London, England, pp. 139–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zec, R. F., Parks, R. W., Gambach, J., and Vicari, S. (1992). The Executive Board System: An innovative approach to cognitive-behavioral rehabilitation in patients with traumatic brain injury. In Long, C. J., and Ross, L. K. (eds.),Handbook of Head Trauma: Acute Care to Recovery Plenum, New York, pp. 219–230.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Parks, R.W., Levine, D.S., Long, D.L. et al. Parallel distributed processing and neuropsychology: A neural network model of Wisconsin card sorting and verbal fluency. Neuropsychol Rev 3, 213–233 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01108843

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01108843

Key words

Navigation