Skip to main content
Log in

Reading-disabled hyperactive children: A distinct subgroup of Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity?

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Mixed hyperactive/reading-disabled children, selected from a large sample of hyperactive children, were compared to pure hyperactive children on demographic, behavioral, and neuropsychological measures. Children in the Mixed group were reading-disabled relative to age and IQ; those in the Pure group had achievement scores that were average relative to age and IQ. The Mixed group was significantly older than the Pure group; there were no differences in gender, race, or socioeconomic status. Behaviorally, the groups did not differ significantly on teacher ratings or on psychiatric ratings of aggression. The Mixed group had a significantly higher Performance IQ, whereas the Pure group had a significantly higher Verbal IQ and performed better on measures of cognitive impulsivity. Several other measures failed to distinguish the groups. The data give minimal support to the notion that pure hyperactive and mixed hyperative/reading-disabled children constitute distinct subgroups of Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, P. T., Dykman, R. A., Holcomb, P. J., & McCray, D. S. (1982). Methylphenidate effects on cognitive style and reaction time in four groups of children.Psychiatry Research, 7, 199–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman, P. T., Dykman, R. A., & Peters, J. E. (1976). Hierarchical factor patterns on the WISC as related to areas of learning deficit.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 583–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, T. W. (1975). Patterns of attention to temporal stimulus sequences and their relationship to reading achievement.Child Development, 46, 1031–1038.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, D. J. (1967). Temporal order, meaningfulness, and reading ability.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 24 1027–1030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beery, K. E. (1967).Developmental test of visual-motor integration. Chicago: Follett.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belmont, L., & Birch, H. G. (1966). The intellectual profile of retarded readers.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 22, 787–816.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantwell, D. P., & Satterfield, J. H. (1978). The prevalence of academic underachievement in hyperactive children.Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 161–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cone, T. E., & Wilson, L. R. (1981). Quantifying a severe discrepancy: A critical analysis.Learning Disability Quarterly, 4, 359–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K. (1969). A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies with children.American Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 884–888.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., Rudel, R. G., & Broman, M. (1980). The development of spatial orientation skills in normal, learning-disabled and neurologically impaired children. In D. Caplan D. Caplan (Ed.),Biological studies of mental processes (p. 44–59). Cambridge: M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gittelman, R., Abikoff, H., Pollack, E., Klein, D. F., Katz, S., & Mattes, J. (1980). A controlled trial of behavior modification and methylphenidate in hyperactive children. In C. Whalen & B. Henker (Eds.),Hyperactive children: The social ecology of identification and treatment (pp. 221–243) New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gittelman-Klein, R., Klein, D. F., Katz, S., Saraf, K., & Pollack, E. (1976). Comparative effects of methylphenidate and thioridazine in hyperkinetic children.Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 1217–1231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. (1958).Social class and mental illness. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, J. F., & Jastak, S. R. (1978).The Wide Range Achievement Test. Wilmington: Jastak Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keogh, B. K. (1971). Hyperactivity and learning disorders: Review and speculation. Exceptional Children,38, 101–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, S. A., McCarthy, J. J., & Kirk, W. D. (1968).Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, N. M., & Sandoval, J. (1980). The prevalence of learning disabilities in a sample of children considered hyperactive.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loney, J., & Milich, R. (1981). Hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression in clinical practice. In M. Wolraich & D. K. Routh (Eds.),Advances in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 2). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milich, R., & Loney, J. (1979). The role of hyperactive and aggressive symptomatology in predicting adolescent outcome among hyperactive children.Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 4, 93–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, F. W., Adaras, P. A., Forrest, T., Stolz, L. M., & Fisher, S. (1971). Learning disorders in children: Sibling studies.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 36 (Serial No. 144, 1–77).

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, C. R. (1981). The fallacy of “two years below grade level for age” as a diagnostic criterion for reading disorders.Journal of School Psychology, 19, 350–358.

    Google Scholar 

  • Routh, D. K. (1980). Developmental and social aspects of hyperactivity. In C. R. Whalen & B. Henker (Eds.),Hyperactive children: The social ecology of indentification and treatment. (pp. 55–73). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & Yule, W. (1975). The concept of specific reading retardation.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 181–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schachar, R., Rutter, M., & Smith, A. (1981). The characteristics of situationally and pervasively hyperactive children: Implications for syndrome definition.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.22, 375–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. A., Cummings, C., Singer, S., & DeBlois, C. S. (1981). The overlap between hyperactive and unsocialized aggressive children.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 35–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. A., DeBlois, C. S., & Cummings, C. (1980). Psychiatric disorder in the parents of hyperactive boys and those with conduct disorder.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 21, 283–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sturge, C. (1982). Reading retardation and antisocial behaviour.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23, 21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symmes, J. S., & Rapoport, J. L. (1972). Unexpected reading failure.American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 42, 82–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. R. (1967). The incidence of verbal disability associated with retardation reading.Neuropsychologia, 5, 175–179.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported in part by Mental Health Clinical Research Grant MH-30906 and by U. S. Public Health Service Grant MH-18579.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Halperin, J.M., Gittelman, R., Klein, D.F. et al. Reading-disabled hyperactive children: A distinct subgroup of Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity?. J Abnorm Child Psychol 12, 1–14 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00913458

Download citation

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00913458

Keywords

Navigation