Skip to main content
Log in

How stalk-eyed flies eye stalk-eyed flies: Observations and measurements of the eyes ofCyrtodiopsis whitei (Diopsidae, Diptera)

  • Published:
Journal of comparative physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

  1. 1.

    The Malayan stalk-eyed flyCyrtodiopsis whitei, of the family Diopsidae, exhibits sexual dimorphism in that the eyestalk span of the males exceeds the length of the body, whereas that of the females, which have shorter bodies on the average, is smaller than the body length. The ratio of the sexes in a population departs significantly from 1∶1; in samples of newly emerged imagines there are twice as many females as males.

  2. 2.

    The behaviour ofCyrtodiopsis is very much determined by vision. During the day, temporary territories may be defended by threat behaviour. At dusk the animals gather in small groups on selected threadlike structures, returning to the same site each day. When males of about equal size encounter one another within such a group they may engage in ritualized fights (or occasionally contact fights). Competitors are driven away by the dominant male. Conspecifics are most likely to elicit a threat or flight reaction when they are at a distance of about 50 mm, and reactions to model flies and reflections in a mirror also occur at about this critical distance.

  3. 3.

    Body length is closely correlated with both eyestalk span and the number of ommatidia in the compound eyes (cf. Results, Sect. 2, Table 1 and Figs. 2, 3, 7 and 8). The largest animals have about 2,600 ommatidia in each eye; the number of optic nerve fibres in the eyestalk is about 6,600.

  4. 4.

    Each compound eye sees a region of space extending over more than a hemisphere in all directions, so that there is extensive binocular overlap; about 70% of the ommatidia of each eye have binocular partner ommatidia in the opposite eye viewing in the same direction.

    The binocular field is most extensive in the frontoventral quadrant, where it reaches over 135 °, and is smallest in the dorsal region.

  5. 5.

    In large animals the size of the binocular field increases because of the greater number of ommatidia. This increase affects chiefly the frontoventral quadrant. As a result, the blind region immediately ahead of and below the head is independent of body size, extending to a distance of only 3 mm in the median plane.

  6. 6.

    The divergence angles of adjacent ommatidia are ca. 3 ° in most parts of the eye. In the foveal region this angle is reduced to just over 1 °. The optical axes of the foveal ommatidia point forward in the horizontal plane; the foveal zone is above and adjacent to the region of greatest binocular overlap. In a small part of the lateral monocular region the divergence angles increase to 5 °.

  7. 7.

    Optomotor experiments show that the head can be moved actively by ca. ±25 ° about the vertical and longitudinal axes, and 20 ° upward and 35 ° downward about the eyestalk axis. Conspecifics are fixated by turning the head and body about the vertical and transverse axes, so as to center the object in the binocular field, either in the horizontal plane or in the adjacent region of greatest binocular overlap. During a ritualized fight the opponents keep their eyestalks parallel and position themselves such that each animal sees the other in a region between the horizontal plane (fovea) and a plane inclined frontoventrad (in the region of greatest binocular overlap), at a distance equal to or smaller than the eyestalk span.

  8. 8.

    The way conspecifics of various sizes at various distances are imaged in the ommatidial array is calculated for the horizontal plane and a plane tilted downward in front by 20 °. Each combination of size and distance generates a different pattern in the array.

  9. 9.

    The pattern constellations on the ommatidial array, like the behavioural observations, indicate that the size and distance of a conspecific can be detected over a relatively extensive region, from a few millimeters to a meter away from the viewing animal. The high foveal resolution, the wide separation of the eyes and the large binocular field make this possible.

  10. 10.

    The eyestalks are probably intraspecific sign stimuli by which a conspecific is recognized and its size (and hence the strength of a potential opponent) is determined.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bauer Th (1981) Prey capture and structure of the visual space of an insect that hunts by sight on the litter layer (Notiophilus biguttatus F., Carabidae, Coleoptera). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 8:91–97

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer Th (1983) Adaptation of compound eyes and capture behaviour to visual hunting in ground beetles (Coleoptera, Carabidae). Zoomorphology (in press)

  • Burkhardt D (1972) Electrophysiological studies on the compound eye of a stalked-eye fly,Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni (Diopsidae, Diptera). J Comp Physiol 81:203–214

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt D, Motte I de la (1980)Cyrtodiopsis spec. (Diptera, Diopsidae) — Kommentkampf. Encyclopaedia Cinematographica, Göttingen, E 2671

  • Burkhardt D, Motte I de la (1981a) Optisch gesteuerte Verhaltensweisen von StielaugenfliegenCyrtodiopsis spec. (Diopsidae, Diptera): 1. Kommentkampf. Verh Dtsch Zool Ges 1981:213

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt D, Motte I de la (1981 b)Cyrtodiopsis spec. (Diptera, Diopsidae) — Schlüpfen aus dem Puparium — Ausformung der Augenstiele. Encyclopaedia Ciaematographica, Göttingen, E 2668

  • Burkhardt D, Motte I de la (1982)Cyrtodiopsis spec. (Diptera, Diopsidae) — Putzverhalten. Encyclopaedia Cinematographica, Göttingen, E 2695

  • Burkhardt D, Motte I de la, Seitz G (1965) Physiological optics of the compound eye of the blow fly. In: Bernhard CG (ed) The functional organization of the compound eye. Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 51–62

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt D, Darnhofer-Demar B, Fischer K (1973) Zum binokularen Entfernungssehen der Insekten. I. Die Struktur des Sehraums von Synsekten. J Comp Physiol 87: 165–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Collett TS (1978) Peering — a locust behaviour pattern for obtaining motion parallax information. J Exp Biol 76:237–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Collett TS, Land MF (1975) Visual control of flight behaviour in the hoverfly,Syritta pipiens L. J Comp Physiol 99:1–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Eggers F (1925) Diopsiden aus Deutsch-Ostafrika. Zool Jahrb Abt Syst Oekol Geogr Tiere 49:469–500

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson ES (1980) Movement parallax and distance perception in the grasshopper (Phaulacridiumvittatum (Sjöstedt). J Exp Biol 86:337–340

    Google Scholar 

  • Hennig W (1965) Die Acalyptratae des Baltischen Bernsteins. Stuttg Beitr Naturkd 145:1–215

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooke R (1665) Micrographia or some physiological descriptions of minute bodies made by the magnifying glasses with observations and inquiries thereupon. Martyn J, Allestry J, London

    Google Scholar 

  • McAlpine DK (1975) Combat between males ofPogonortalis doclea (Diptera, Platystomidae) and its relation to structural modification. Aust Entomol Mag 2:104–107

    Google Scholar 

  • McAlpine DK (1979) Agonistic behaviour inAchias australis (Diptera, Platystomidae) and the significance of eyestalks. In: Blum MS, Blum NA (eds) Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects. Academic Press, London, pp 221–230

    Google Scholar 

  • Motte I de la, Burkhardt D (1983) Portrait of an Asian stalkeyed fly,Cyrtodiopsis whitei Curran (Diopsidae, Acalyptrata, Diptera). Naturwissenschaften (in press)

  • Seitz G, Burkhardt D (1974) Bau und optische Leistungen des Komplexauges der StielaugenfliegeCyrtodiopsis dalmanni Wiedemann. J Comp Physiol 95:49–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Shillito JF (1960) A bibliography of the Diopsidae. J Soc Bibliogr Nat Hist 3:337–350

    Google Scholar 

  • Shillito JF (1971) Dimorphism in flies with stalked eyes. Zool J Linn Soc 50:297–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Shillito JF (1974) ‘Paradoxum Insectum’ — Linnaeus onDiopsis (Insecta: Diptera). Biol J Linn Soc 6:277–287

    Google Scholar 

  • Shillito JF (1976) Bibliography of the Diopsidae-II. J Soc Bibliogr Nat Hist 8:65–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan KB (1965) The taxonomy, biology, ecology and behaviour of some Malayan Diopsidae (Diptera). Thesis, Univ Malaya, Kuala Lumpur

    Google Scholar 

  • Uexküll J von, Brock F (1927) Atlas zur Bestimmung der Orte in den Sehräumen der Tiere. Z Vgl Physiol 5:167–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Via SE (1977) Visually mediated snapping in the Bulldog Ant: A perceptual ambiguity between size and distance. J Comp Physiol 121:33–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace GK (1959) Visual scanning in the Desert LocustSchistocerca gregaria Forskål. J Exp Biol 36:512–525

    Google Scholar 

  • Wehner R (1975) Pattern Recognition. In: Horridge GA (ed) The compound eye and vision of insects. Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 75–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Wehner R (1981) Spatial vision in arthropods. In: Autrum H (ed) Comparative physiology and evolution of vision in invertebrates. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York (Handbook of sensory physiology, vol VII/6C, pp 285–616)

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickler W, Seibt U (1972) Zur Ethologie afrikanischer Stielaugenfliegen (Diptera, Diopsidae). Z Tierphysiol 31:113–130

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Burkhardt, D., de la Motte, I. How stalk-eyed flies eye stalk-eyed flies: Observations and measurements of the eyes ofCyrtodiopsis whitei (Diopsidae, Diptera). J. Comp. Physiol. 151, 407–421 (1983). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00605457

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00605457

Keywords

Navigation