Skip to main content
Log in

The random elements in the systematic search behavior of the desert isopod Hemilepistus reaumuri

  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

Most of the year the isopod crustacean Hemilepistus reaumuri depends for survival on the protection of a permanent shelter (i.e., the burrow occupied by its family), despite its remarkable physiological adaptations to its desert habitat. If an isopod gets lost after an excursion from the burrow, it has to find it as quickly as possible.

Displacement experiments show that H. reaumuri is indeed successful in homing. Even if a desert isopod is displaced in an arbitrary direction over a distance (starting distance r 0) from the burrow that exceeds 15 times the distance from which it can detect the entrance, it returns on average in less than 400 s.

The homing behavior of H. reaumuri is successful not because of the isopod's ability to navigate to its burrow by using external orientation stimuli but because of the intrinsic structure of its search pattern. An isopod that is displaced from its burrow first searches approximately in the form of a spiral, then it moves through increasing loops on which meanders are superimposed. It concentrates these subunits of its search around the starting point by occasionally returning there. The time course of the azimuthal direction component of its position is not clearly regular. The surroundings of the starting point are searched evenly in every direction.

The search behavior of H. reaumuri is composed of systematic subunits together with many random elements. The form of discrete brownian (random) search without directional correlations between its steps that best describes the observed behavior of H. reaumuri for search-path segments with a length between 1 and 7 m has an average step length of 33 cm. For segments with a searchpath length below 1 m the agreement between theory and observations is better if one starts from a discrete brownian search with a much smaller step length (one body length), but in which the direction of the steps is strongly correlated.

Despite these geometrical similarities to a brownian search the search behavior of H. reaumuri is distinctly more successful because of the combination of two characteristics. H. reaumuri avoids the disadvantage of the most successful form of a brownian search (i.e., the frequent passage through a region in which it has searched just before) by moving in straighter lines. A brownian search with the same directional constancy shown by H. reaumuri would be inefficient because the thoroughness with which a given region is searched would be too low. H. reaumuri avoids this problem and concentrates its search around the starting point by sometimes returning to that place.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Edney EB (1968) Transition from water to land in isopod crustaceans. Am Zool 8:309–326

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerritsen J, Strickler JR (1977) Encounter probabilities and community structure in zooplankton: A mathematical model. J Fish Res Board Can 34:73–82

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin DR (1952) Bird navigation. Biol Rev 27:359–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Heinroth O, Heinroth K (1941) Das Heimfindevermögen der Brieftauben. J Ornithol 89:213–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodge CF (1894) The method of homing pigeons. Pop Sci Monthly 44:758–775

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann G (1978) Experimentelle and theoretische Analyse eines adaptiven Orientierungsverhaltens: Die ‘optimale’ Suche der Wüstenassel Hemilepistus reaumuri, Audouin and Savigny (Crustacea, Isopoda, Oniscoidea) nach ihrer Höhle. Ph D thesis, Regensburg

  • Hoffmann G (1983) The search behavior of the desert isopod Hemilepistus reaumuri as compared with a systematic search. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 13:93–106

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann G (1983a) Homing by a systematic search. Proc 8. Cybern Congr (in press)

  • Koopman BO (1946) Search and screening. Operations Evaluation Group Rep 56, Washington, DC

  • Kramer E (1976) The orientation of walking honeybees in odour fields with small concentration gradients. Physiol Entomol 1:27–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Laing J (1938) Host finding by insect parasites. II. The chance of Trichogramma evanescens finding its hosts. J Exp Biol 15:281–302

    Google Scholar 

  • Lincoln RJ (1970) A review of the species of Hemilepistus s. str. Budde — Lund 1885 (Isopoda, Porcellionidae). Bull Br Mus Zool 20:109–130

    Google Scholar 

  • Linsenmair KE (1972) Die Bedeutung familienspezifischer Abzeichen für den Familienzusammenhalt bei der sozialen Wüstenassel Hemilepistus reaumuri Audouin u. Savigny (Crystacea, Isopoda, Oniscoidea). Z Tierpsychol 31:131–162

    Google Scholar 

  • Linsenmair KE (1975) Some adaptions of the desert woodlouse Hemilepistus reaumuri (Isopoda, Oniscoidea) to desert environment. Verh Ges Oekol (Erlangen) 1975:183–185

    Google Scholar 

  • Linsenmair KE (1979) Untersuchungen zur Soziobiologie der Wüstenassel Hemilepistus reaumuri und verwandter Isopodenarten (Isopoda, Oniscoidea): Paarbindung und Evolution der Monogamie. Verh Dtsch Zool Ges 1979:60–72

    Google Scholar 

  • Linsenmair KE, Linsenmair C (1971) Paarbildung u. Paarzusammenhalt bei der monogamen Wüstenassel Hemilepistus reaumuri (Crustacea, Isopoda, Oniscoidea). Z Tierpsychol 29:134–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Mardia KV (1972) Statistics of directional data. Academic Press, London New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews GVT (1971) Vogelflug. Goldmann, München

    Google Scholar 

  • Paloheimo JE (1971) On a theory of search. Biometrika 58:61–75

    Google Scholar 

  • Patten BC (1964) The rational decision process in salmon migration. J Cons Int Explor Mer 28:410–417

    Google Scholar 

  • Pretzl R (1976) Vergleichende Untersuchungen zur Transpiration und aktiven Wasserabgabe der Wüstenassel Hemilepistus reaumuri (Audouin u. Savigny) und der Kellerassel Porcellio scaber (Latreille). Dissertation, Universität Regensburg

  • Rennie J (1835) The faculties of birds. London

  • Rogers DJ (1972) Random search and insect population models. J Anim Ecol 41:369–383

    Google Scholar 

  • Saila SB, Shappy RA (1963) Random movement and orientation in salmon migration. J Cons Int Explor Mer 28:153–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Schöne H (1980) Orientierung im Raum. Wiss Verlags-Ges, Stuttgart

    Google Scholar 

  • Seelinger G (1977) Der Antennenendzapfen der tunesischen Wüstenassel Hemilepistus reaumuri, ein komplexes Sinnesorgan (Crustacea, Isopoda). J Comp Physiol 113:95–103

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanley J (1932) A mathematical theory of the growth of populations of the flour beetle Tribolium confusum Duv. Can J Res 6:632–671

    Google Scholar 

  • Tchen CM (1952) Random flight with multiple partial correlations. J Chem Phys 20:214–217

    Google Scholar 

  • Varley GC (1941) On the search for hosts and the egg distribution of some chalcid parasits of the knapweed gall-fly. Parasitology 33:47–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Wehner R, Srinivasan MV (1981) Searching behaviour of desert ants, genus Cataglyphis (Formicidae, Hymenoptera). J Comp Physiol 142:315–338

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson DH (1952) The random element in bird ‘navigation’. J Exp Biol 29:532–560

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hoffmann, G. The random elements in the systematic search behavior of the desert isopod Hemilepistus reaumuri . Behav Ecol Sociobiol 13, 81–92 (1983). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00293798

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00293798

Keywords

Navigation