Skip to main content
Log in

Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic character of masculine generics

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This experiment investigated the propensity of the generic he to evoke images of males relative to he/she and the plural they. Undergraduates read sentences aloud and verbally described the images that came to mind. The results provide strong support for the hypothesis that the generic he evokes a disproportionate number of male images. Results also suggest that while the plural they functions as a generic pronoun for both males and females, males may comprehend he/she in a manner similar to he. Theoretical implications for a critique of sexist language and prescribing generic pronoun usage are considered.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • APA Publication Manual Task Force. (1977). Guidelines for nonsexist language in APA journals: Publication Manual change sheet 2. American Psychologist, 32, 487–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S., & Bem, D. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising “aid and abet” sex discrimination? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 3, 6–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bendix, E. (1979). Linguistic model as political symbols: Gender and the generic “he” in English. In J. Orasanu, M. Slater, & L. Adler (Eds.), Language, sex and gender: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 327). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaubergs, M. (1980). An analysis of classic arguments against changing sexist language. In C. Kramarae (Ed.), The voice of women and men. New York: Permagon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodine, A. (1975). Androcentrism in prescriptive grammar: Singular “they,” sex-indefinite “he” and “he or she.” Language in Society, 4, 129–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briere, J., & Lanktree, C. (1983). Sex-role related effects of sex bias in language. Sex Roles, 9, 625–632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. (1983). Sexist language in occupational information: Does it make a difference? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23, 227–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, C., Hill, F., & Dayley, L. (1983). Do masculine pronouns used generically lead to thoughts of men? Sex Roles, 9, 737–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R. (1977). Language, history, and the legal process: A profile of the “reasonable man.” Camden Law Journal, 8, 312–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Creech, B., & Wilson, R. (1979). Occupational stereotyping and the sexist effects of pronouns. Unpublished manuscript, North Carolina State University.

  • Dillard, J. (1989). Unpublished raw data. University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI.

  • Faggen-Steckler, J., McCarthy, K., & Tittle, C. (1974). A quantitative method for measuring sex “bias” in standardized tests. Journal of Educational Measurement, 11, 151–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, A. (1980). Notes on the tyranny of language usage. In C. Kramarae (Ed.), The voices of women and men. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, A. (1973). The making of a nonsexist dictionary. In B. Thorne & N. Henley (Eds.), Language and sex: Difference and dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, L. (1975). Cro-magnon woman—In eclipse. Science Teacher, 42, 8–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilbrun, A. (1981). Human sex-role behavior. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. (1986). Mother tongue, father time. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, V. (1971). A study of the images produced through the use of male pronouns as the generic. Moments in Contemporary Rhetoric and Communications, 1, 25–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramarae, C. (1981). Women and men speaking. Rowley, MA: Newbury House Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. (1980a). On the goals, principles, and procedures for prescriptive grammar. Language and society, 9, 349–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. (1980b). Psychology, prescriptive grammar, and the pronoun problem. American Psychologist, 35, 444–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. (1983). Prescriptive grammar and the pronoun problem. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender and society. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D., & Fulkerson, D. (1979). On the comprehension and production of pronouns. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 661–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martyna, W. (1978). What does “he” mean? Use of the generic masculine. Journal of Communication, 28, 131–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martyna, W. (1983). Beyond the “he/man” approach: The case for nonsexist language. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender, and society. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moulton, J., Robinson, G., & Cherin, E. (1978). Psychology in action: Sex bias in language use: “Neutral” pronouns that aren't. American Psychologist, 33, 1032–1036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nilsen, A. (1977). Sexism in children's books and elementary teaching materials. In A. Nilsen, H. Bosmajian, H. Gershuny, & J. Stanley (Eds.), Sexism and language. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pincus, A., & Pincus, R. (1980). Linguistic sexism and career education. Language Arts, 57, 70–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, M. (1975). Alice through the statues. McGill Law Journal, 21.

  • Schneider, J., & Hacker, S. (1973). Sex-role imagery and use of the generic “man” in introductory texts: A case in the sociology of sociology. The American Sociologist, 8, 12–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheplack, N., Ogden, D., & Tobin-Bennett, D. (1976). Students' perceptions of career as a function of gender cues. Unpublished paper, Indiana University.

  • Silveira, J. (1980). Generic masculine words and thinking. In C. Kramarae (Ed.), The voice of women and men. New York: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sniezek, J., & Jazwinski, C. (1986). Gender bias in English: In search of fair language. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16(7), 642–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soto, D. (1976, November). Alternatives to using masculine pronouns when referring to the species. Paper presented at the 46th annual meeting of the Western Speech Communication Association, San Francisco.

  • Stericker, A. (1981). Does this “he or she” business really make a difference? The effect of masculine pronouns as generics on job attitudes. Sex Roles, 7, 637–641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strunk, W., & White, E. B. (1979). The elements of style. New York: MacMillan Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Switzer, J. Y. (1990). The impact of generic word choices: an empirical investigation of age- and sex-related differences. Sex Roles, 22, 69–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whorf, B. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd-Mancillas, W. (1981). Masculine generics = sexist language. Communication Quarterly, 29(2), 107–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd-Mancillas, W., & Meyers, K. (1980, May). The effects of inclusive/exclusive language on reading comprehension, perceived human interest, and likelihood of inclusive pronoun usage. Paper presented at the 30th annual International Communication Association, Acapulco, Mexico.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

I would like to thank Lea Haravon and Jim Dillard for their assistance in the design and completion of this study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gastil, J. Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles 23, 629–643 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289252

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289252

Keywords

Navigation