Skip to main content
Log in

Conformorality. A Study on Group Conditioning of Normative Judgment

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

How does other people’s opinion affect judgments of norm transgressions? In our study, we used a modification of the famous Asch paradigm (1951, 1955) to examine conformity in the moral domain. The question we addressed was how peer group opinion alters normative judgments of scenarios involving violations of moral, social, and decency norms. The results indicate that even moral norms are subject to conformity, especially in situations with a high degree of social presence. Interestingly, the degree of conformity can distinguish between different types of norms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In what follows, ‘social convention’ is used as a synonym for ‘social norm.’

  2. Interestingly, also for Turiel, emotions are prominent aspects of moral norms. Reporting on children’s reactions to different norm transgressions, Turiel (1977) writes: “The feedback in the context of moral transgressions generally focused on the effects of actions upon others and on emotional reactions. In contrast, the feedback in the context of social-conventional transgressions focused on aspects of social order, such as rules, sanctions, and norm violations” (Turiel 1977, p. 110; see also Nucci and Turiel 1978).

References

  • Asch, S. 1951. Effects of group pressure on the modification and distortion of judgments. In Groups, leadership and men, ed. H. Guetzkow, 177–190. Pittsburgh: Carnegie Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. 1955. Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American 193: 33–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C. 2006. The grammar of society. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C. 2008. The fragility of fairness: An experimental investigation on the conditional status of pro-social norms. Philosophical Issues 18: 229–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bordia, P. 1997. Face-to-face versus computer-mediated communication: A synthesis of the experimental literature. The Journal of Business Communication 34: 99–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J.M., S. Soroka, K. Gonzago, E. Murphy, and E. Somervell. 2001. The effect of fleeting attraction on compliance to requests. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27: 1578–1586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R.B., and N.J. Goldstein. 2004. Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology 55: 591–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R., C. Kallgren, and R. Reno. 1991. A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 24: 201–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cinnirella, M., and B. Green. 2007. Does ‘cyber-conformity’ vary cross-culturally? Exploring the effect of culture and communication medium on social conformity. Computers in Human Behavior 23: 2011–2025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. 1990. Norms of revenge. Ethics 100: 826–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. 2009. Social norms and the explanation of behavior. In The Oxford handbook of analytical sociology, ed. P. Hedström and P. Bearman, 195–217. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J.D., R. Sommerville, L. Nystrom, J. Darley, and J. Cohen. 2001. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293: 2105–2108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J.D., L.E. Nystrom, A.D. Engell, J.M. Darley, and J.D. Cohen. 2004. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44: 389–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108: 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., S. Koller, and M. Dias. 1993. Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 613–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and R. Boyd. 1998. The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution & Human Behavior 19: 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hiltz, S.R., K. Johnson, and M. Turoff. 1986. Experiments in group decision making communication process and outcome in face-to-face versus computerized conferences. Human Communication Research 13: 225–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogg, M.A., and S.A. Reid. 2006. Social identity, self-categorization, and the communication of group norms. Communication Theory 16: 7–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, B., S. Dwyer, and M. Hauser. 2009. The role of emotion in moral psychology. Trends in Cognitive Science 13: 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D. 2011. Yuck! The nature and moral significance of disgust. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D., S. Stich, K. Haley, S. Eng, and D. Fessler. 2007. Harm, affect and the moral/conventional distinction. Mind and Language 22: 117–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knobe, J. 2003. Intentional action and side effects in ordinary language. Analysis 63: 190–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laporte, L., C. van Nimwegen, and A.J. Uyttendaele. 2010. Do people say what they think: Social conformity behavior in varying degrees of online social presence. In Proceedings of NordiCHI 2010, eds. E.B. Hvannberg, M.K. Lárusdóttir, A. Blandford and J. Gulliksen, 305–314.

  • Milgram, S. 1963. Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 67: 371–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nado, J., D. Kelly, and S. Stich. 2009. Moral judgment. In The Routledge companion to the philosophy of psychology, ed. J. Symons and P. Calvo, 621–633. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. 2002. Norms with feeling: Towards a psychological account of moral judgment. Cognition 84: 221–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. 2004. Sentimental rules: On the natural foundations of moral judgment. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L. 2001. Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nucci, L., and E. Turiel. 1978. Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development 49: 400–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. 2006. The emotional basis of moral judgments. Philosophical Explorations 9: 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puntoni, S., B. de Langhe, and S. Van Osselaer. 2008. Bilingualism and the emotional intensity of advertising language. Journal of Consumer Research 35: 1012–1025.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royzman, E., R. Leeman, and J. Baron. 2009. Unsentimental ethics: Towards a content-specific account of the moral-conventional distinction. Cognition 112: 159–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Royzman, E.B., G.P. Goodwin, and R.F. Leeman. 2011. When sentimental rules collide: Norms with feelings in the dilemmatic context. Cognition 121: 101–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., M. Markwith, and C.R. McCauley. 1994. Sensitivity to indirect contacts with other persons: AIDS aversion as a composite of aversion to strangers, infection, moral taint and misfortune. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 103: 495–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., J. Haidt, G.L. Clore, and A.H. Jordan. 2008. Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34: 1096–1109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, P.W., J.M. Nolan, R.B. Cialdini, N.J. Goldstein, and V. Griskevicius. 2007. The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological Science 18: 429–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Short, J., E. Williams, and B. Christie. 1976. The social psychology of telecommunications. London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, P. 1972. Famine, affluence, and morality. Philosophy and Public Affairs 1: 229–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smetana, J. 1993. Understanding of social rules. In The development of social cognition: The child as psychologist, ed. M. Bennett. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smilowitz, M., C. Compton, and L. Flint. 1988. The effect of computer mediated communication on an individual’s judgement: A study based on the methods of Asch’s social influence experiment. Computers in Human Behavior 4: 311–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sousa, P. 2009. On testing the ‘moral law’. Mind & Language 24: 209–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sousa, P., C. Holbrook, and J. Piazza. 2009. The morality of harm. Cognition 113: 80–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stich, S., D. Fessler, and D. Kelly. 2009. On the morality of harm: A response to Sousa, Holbrook and Piazza. Cognition 113(1): 93–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., M. Billig, R. Bundy, and C. Flament. 1971. Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology 1: 149–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. 1977. Distinct conceptual and developmental domains: Social convention and morality. In Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, eds. H. Howe and C. Keasey Lincoln, Social Cognitive Development, 25, 77–116. University of Nebraska Press.

  • Turiel, E. 1983. The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turiel, E. 2002. The culture of morality: Social development, context and conflict. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lankveld, G., P. Spronck, J. van den Herik, and A. Arntz. 2011. Games as personality profiling tools. In Proceedings of the 2011 I.E. Conference on Computational Intelligence in Games (CIG’11), ed. M. Preuss, 197–202.

  • Wheatley, T., and J. Haidt. 2005. Hypnotically induced disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science 16: 780–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., V. Horner, and F.B. de Waal. 2005. Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437: 737–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Hans Westerbeek, Stephan Hartmann, Natalie Gold, Francesco Guala, Edouard Machery, Henrik Singmann, Carel van Wijk and Eric Postma as well as to the audiences of the following workshops and conferences where the paper was presented, including:

• Philosophy, Politics and Economics Talk, University of Pennsylvania. USA, March 2012

• Rotterdam-TilburgWorkshop in Philosophy of Science. Tilburg University, The Netherlands, November 2011

• Workshop on Formal Epistemology Meets Experimental Philosophy, Tilburg University, The Netherlands, September 2011

• Workshop on Morality and the Cognitive Sciences. University of Latvia, Latvia, May 2011

MC is grateful to the British Society for the Philosophy of Science and to the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft-priority program “New Frameworks of Rationality” (SPP 1516) for financial support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matteo Colombo.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lisciandra, C., Postma-Nilsenová, M. & Colombo, M. Conformorality. A Study on Group Conditioning of Normative Judgment. Rev.Phil.Psych. 4, 751–764 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-013-0161-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-013-0161-4

Keywords

Navigation