Skip to main content
Log in

Attention in Sentence Processing: A Review

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Psychological Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This review looks at the relationship between attention and language. There have been many studies that have shown a certain role of attention on sentence and spoken word processing. At the same time some other studies have observed that language processing is autonomous. In this review an attempt has been made to review the empirical evidence from sentence processing in reading, auditory processing and speaking domains to see if attentional manipulations cause interference with processing. The review suggests currently available evidence is rather mixed and it is still a long way to fully explore the attentional basis of sentence processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). Joint attention and vocabulary development: a criticallook. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1(3), 195–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenthal, A. L. (1970). Language and psychology: historical aspects of psycholinguistics. M. S. Mirón (Ed.). New York: Wiley.

  • Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. A., Cavanagh, P., Chun, M. M., & Nakayama, K. (2012). The attentional requirements of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 411–417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., et al. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron, 21(4), 761–773.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (1999). Form and content: dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron, 24(2), 427–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: a dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112, 777–813.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Finneran, D., Francis, A. L., & Leonard, L. (2009). Sustained attention in preschool-age children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 915–929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Pedrolli, K., & Facoetti, A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Current Biology, 22(9), 814–815. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., & Fodor, J. D. (1978). The sausage machine: a new two-stage parsing model. Cognition, 6(4), 291–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., Kotz, S. A., Werheid, K., Hein, G., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). Syntactic comprehension in Parkinson’s disease: investigating early automatic and late integrational processes using event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychology, 17(1), 133–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y., & Friederici, A. D. (2006). Neuroimaging of syntax and syntactic processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., & Zimmermann, P. (1999). A wider view of attachment and exploration: stability and change during the years of immaturity. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 760–786). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1996). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis : early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D. (1999). Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (1990). Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 57(6), 787–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphries, C., Willard, K., Buchsbaum, B., & Hickok, G. (2001). Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(8), 1749–1752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Inhoff, A. W. (1989). Parafoveal processing of words and saccade computation during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 544.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1996). The architecture of the language faculty (Vol. 28). MIT Press.

  • Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., Keller, T. A., Eddy, W. F., & Thulborn, K. R. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science, 274, 114–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, E., Harris, A., Gibson, E., & Holcomb, P. (2000). The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15(2), 159–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Chajczyk, D. (1983). Tests of the automaticity of reading: dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 9, 497–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, A., & Pynte, J. (2005). Parafoveal-on-foveal effects in normal reading. Vision Research, 45(2), 153–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kristensen, L. B., Wang, L., Petersson, K. M., & Hagoort, P. (2012). The interface between language and attention: prosodic focus marking recruits a general attention network in spoken language comprehension. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kumar, U., Padakannaya, P., Mishra, R. K., & Khetrapal, C. L. (2012). Distinctive neural signatures for negative sentences in Hindi: an fMRI study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 1–11.

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M. and Van Petten, C (1994). Psycholinguistics Electrified: Event-related potential investigations. In: M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Academic Press, pp. 83–143.

  • Levelt, W. (1989). Speaking: from intention to articulation Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12(4), 523–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1989). Automaticity and cognitive control. Unintended Thought, 52–74.

  • Luck, S. J., Woodman, G. F., & Vogel, E. K. (2000). Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 432–440.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maidhof, C., & Koelsch, S. (2011). Effects of selective attention on syntax processing in music and language. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2252–2267. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21542.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B., Dubowitz, D. J., et al. (1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 364–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Intelligence, A. (1987). The case for interactionism in language processing.

  • Mesulam, M. (1990). Large–scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Annals of Neurology, 28(5), 597–613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, A. S., Sleiderink, A. M., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1998). Viewing and naming objects: eye movements during noun phrase production. Cognition, 66(2), B25–B33. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00009-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1962). Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist, 17(11), 748–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K. (2009). Interaction of language and visual attention: evidence from production and comprehension. Progress in Brain Research, 176(09), 277–292. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17616-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K. (2012). Studying sentence generation during scene viewing in Hindi with eye tracking. In P. Padakannya & H. Winskel (Eds.), Handbook of South Asian Psycholinguistics. Cambridge University Press.

  • Mishra, R. K., & Marmolejo-Ramos, F. (2010). On the mental representations originating during the interaction between language and vision. Cognitive Processing, 11(4), 295–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., Singh, N., Pandey, A., & Huettig, F. (2012). Spoken language-mediated anticipatory eye movements are modulated by reading ability: Evidence from Indian low and high literates. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5(1), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., Huettig, F., & Olivers, C. N. (2013). Automaticity and conscious decisions during language-mediated eye gaze in the visual world. In N. Srinivasan & V. S. C. Pammi (Eds.), Progress in brain research: decision making: neural and behavioral approaches. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Näätänen, R. (2008). Mismatch negativity (MMN) as an index of central auditory system plasticity. International Journal of Audiology, 47(S2), 16–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 2–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (2012). Attention in a social world. OUP USA.

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y., Hasting, A. S., & Carlyon, R. P. (2008). Syntax as a reflex: neurophysiological evidence for early automaticity of grammatical processing. Brain and Language, 104(3), 244–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radach, R., & Kennedy, A. (2012). Eye movements in reading: some theoretical context. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(3), 1–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rämä, P., Relander-Syrjänen, K., Carlson, S., Salonen, O., & Kujala, T. (2012). Attention and semantic processing during speech: an fMRI study. Brain and Language, 122(2), 114–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Castelhano, M., & Young, J. (2009). Eye movements when looking at unusual/weird scenes: are there culture differences? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 254–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38(4), 850.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogalsky, C., & Hickok, G. (2009). Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19(4), 786–796.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, R. G. (2009). Specific language impairment. Handbook of child language disorders, 3–43.

  • Shepherd, M., Findlay, J. M., & Hockey, R. J. (1986). The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38(3), 475–491.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shtyrov, Y. (2010). Automaticity and attentional control in spoken language processing: neurophysiological evidence. The Mental Lexicon, 5(2), 255–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shtyrov, Y., Smith, M., Horner, A. J., Henson, R., Nathan, P. J., Bullmore, E. T., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Attention to language: Novel MEG paradigm for registering involuntary language processing in the brain. Neuropsychologia.

  • Singh, N. & Mishra, R. K. (submitted). Automatic activation of translation equivalents in Bilinguals leads to interference in a visual task:A visual world eye tracking study with Hindi-English bilinguals.

  • Starr, M. S., & Rayner, K. (2001). Eye movements during reading: some current controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(4), 156–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinhauer, K., & Drury, J. E. (2012). On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120(2), 135–162. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomlin, R. S. (1997). Mapping conceptual representations into linguistic representations: the role of attention in grammar. In J. Nuyts & E. Pederson (Eds.), Language and conceptualization (pp. 162–189). Cambridge: Cambrodge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Traxler, M., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.). (2011). Handbook of psycholinguistics. Academic Press.

  • Trueswell, J. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1994). Toward a lexicalist framework for constraint-based syntactic ambiguity resolution. In C. Clifton, L. Frazier, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Perspectives in sentence processing (pp. 155–179). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 767–773. doi:10.1038/nn1065.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Herten, M., Kolk, H. H., & Chwilla, D. J. (2005). An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(2), 241–255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ramesh Kumar Mishra.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mishra, R.K. Attention in Sentence Processing: A Review. Psychol Stud 58, 308–317 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-013-0197-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-013-0197-7

Keywords

Navigation