Skip to main content
Log in

Managerial decision making in customer management: adaptive, fast and frugal?

  • Original Empirical Research
  • Published:
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While customer management has become a top priority for practitioners and academics, little is known about how managers actually make customer management decisions. Our study addresses this gap and uses the adaptive decision maker as well as the fast and frugal heuristics frameworks to gain a better understanding of managerial decision making. Using the process-tracing tool MouselabWEB, we presented sales managers in retail banking with three typical customer management prediction tasks. The results show that a majority of managers in this study are adaptive in their decision making and that some managers use fast and frugal heuristics. Usage of adaptive decision making seems to be mainly driven by low objective task difficulty, the use of fast and frugal heuristics by experience. While adaptive decision making does not impact predictive accuracy, usage of fast and frugal heuristics is associated with proportionally greater use of high predictive quality cues and a significant increase in accuracy. Hence, the existing skepticism concerning heuristics should be questioned.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Using a cutoff value of 20% was based on the fact that (1) a lower cutoff was not possible since that would have not yielded enough participants to draw meaningful conclusions (only four participants used 10% of the information), (2) a higher cutoff does not seem consistent with the frugal heuristics framework, and (3) the number of participants using 20% or less of the information formed exactly the 10th percentile of all participants. Using the cutoff value of 150 or 300 seconds was based on discussion with the bank’s management. In order to provide stronger support for our findings, we conducted a sensitivity analysis which examined the robustness of our results with respect to different definitions of usage of fast and frugal heuristics. Across different definitions of usage of fast and frugal heuristics based on a range from (1) 14% to 26% (in 2% intervals) for information use, (2) 120 to 180 seconds (in 10 second intervals) for decision time in the low complexity condition, and (3) 270 to 330 seconds for decision time in the high complexity condition, all analyses reported in this article yielded similar results. Thus, our findings are robust to changes in how we operationalize usage of fast and frugal heuristics.

  2. A 2x3 contingency table analyzing the usage of decision strategies by task complexity and by task would not produce reliable results because there were too many cells with an expected value of less than five.

References

  • Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, P. A. (1983). Decision making by objection and the Cuban missile crisis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28(2), 201–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballinger, G. A. (2004). Using generalized estimating equations for longitudinal data analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7(2), 127–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bazerman, M. H. (1998). Judgment in managerial decision making. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. D., & Nasr, N. I. (1998). Customer lifetime value: marketing models and applications. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 12(1), 17–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. D., & Nasr Bechwati, N. I. (2001). The allocation of promotion budget to maximize customer equity. Omega, 29(1), 49–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettman, J. R., Johnson, E. J., Luce, M. F., & Payne, J. W. (1993). Correlation, conflict, and choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(4), 931–951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., & Payne, J. W. (1998). Constructive consumer choice processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(3), 187–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biggs, S. F., Bedard, J. C., Gaber, B. G., & Linsmeier, T. J. (1985). The effects of task size and similarity on the decision behavior of bank loan officers. Management Science, 31(8), 970–987.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blattberg, R. C., & Deighton, J. (1996). Manage marketing by the customer equity test. Harvard Business Review, 74(4), 136–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blattberg, R. C., Getz, G., & Thomas, J. S. (2001). Customer equity: building and managing relationships as valuable assets. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, R. N. (1998). A dynamic model of the duration of the customer’s relationship with a continuous service provider: the role of satisfaction. Marketing Science, 17(1), 45–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breiman, L., Friedman, J., Olshen, R. A., & Stone, C. J. (1993). Classification and regression trees. New York: Chapman & Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bröder, A. (2000). Assessing the empirical validity of the ‘take-the-best’ heuristic as a model of human probabilistic inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1332–1346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bröder, A. (2003). Decision making with the ‘adaptive toolbox’: influence of environmental structure, intelligence, and working memory load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(4), 611–625.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bröder, A., & Schiffer, S. (2003). Take the best versus simultaneous feature matching: probabilistic inferences from memory and effects of representation format. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 277–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camerer, C., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: an experimental approach. American Economic Review, 89(1), 306–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Creyer, E. H., Bettman, J. R., & Payne, J. W. (1990). The impact of accuracy and effort feedback and goals on adaptive decision behavior. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 3(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Academy of Management Review, 32(1), 33–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, D. V., & Lord, R. G. (1992). Expertise and problem categorization: the role of expert processing in organizational sense-making. Journal of Management Studies, 29(1), 35–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dhami, M. K., & Ayton, P. (2001). Bailing and jailing the fast and frugal way. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 14(2), 141–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dwyer, F. R. (1997). Customer lifetime valuation to support marketing decision making. Journal of Direct Marketing, 11(4), 6–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1978). Confidence in judgment: persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 85(5), 395–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenhardt, K. M., & Zbaracki, M. J. (1992). Strategic decision making. Strategic Management Journal, 13(S2), 17–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Kinder, D. R., & Larter, W. M. (1983). The novice and the expert: knowledge-based strategies in political cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(4), 381–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankwick, G. L., Ward, J. C., Hutt, M. D., & Reingen, P. H. (1994). Evolving patterns of organizational beliefs in the formation of strategy. Journal of Marketing, 58(2), 96–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, J. W. (1985). Effects of decision motive and organizational performance level on strategic decision processes. Academy of Management Journal, 28(4), 821–843.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (2004). Fast and frugal heuristics: the tools of bounded rationality. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 62–88). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 451–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103(4), 650–669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., & Selten, R. (2001). Bounded rationality: the adaptive toolbox. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, S., & Zeithaml, V. A. (2006). Customer metrics and their impact on financial performance. Marketing Science, 25(6), 718–739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, S., Lehmann, D. R., & Stuart, J. A. (2004). Valuing customers. Journal of Marketing Research, 41(1), 7–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J., & Hofer, C. W. (1993). Venture capitalists’ decision criteria in new venture evaluation. Journal of Business Venturing, 8(1), 25–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanssens, M., Thorpe, D., & Finkbeiner, C. (2008). Marketing when customer equity matters. Harvard Business Review, 86(5), 117–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hickson, D. J., Butler, R. J., Cray, D., Mallory, G. R., & Wilson, D. C. (1986). Top decisions: strategic decision-making in organizations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoch, S. J., & Schkade, D. A. (1996). A psychological approach to decision support systems. Management Science, 42(1), 51–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffrage, U. (2004). Overconfidence. In R. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions: a handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgment and memory (pp. 235–254). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoving, T. (1996). False impressions: the hunt for big time art fakes. London: Andre Deutsch.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, J. A. (1963). Marketing: executive and buyer behavior. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson, J. W., Alba, J. W., & Eisenstein, E. (2010). Heuristics and biases in data-based decision making: the effects of experience, training, and graphical data displays. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(4), 627–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutt, M. D., Reingen, P. H., & Ronchetto, J. R., Jr. (1988). Tracing emergent processes in marketing strategy formation. Journal of Marketing, 52(1), 4–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isenberg, D. J. (1986). Thinking and managing: a verbal protocol analysis of managerial problem solving. Academy of Management Journal, 29(4), 775–788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jain, D., & Singh, S. S. (2002). Customer lifetime value research in marketing: a review and future directions. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 16(2), 34–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. J., & Meyer, R. J. (1984). Compensatory choice models of noncompensatory processes: the effect of varying context. Journal of Consumer Research, 11(1), 528–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. J., & Payne, J. W. (1985). Effort and accuracy in choice. Management Science, 31(4), 395–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1980). Reasons for confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kumar, V., Lemon, K. N., & Parasuraman, A. (2006). Managing customers for value: an overview and research agenda. Journal of Service Research, 9(2), 87–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liang, K.-Y., & Zeger, S. L. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73(1), 13–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lurigio, A. J., & Carroll, J. S. (1985). Probation officers’ schemata of offenders: content, development, and impact on treatment decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(5), 1112–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahajan, J. (1992). The overconfidence effect in marketing management predictions. Journal of Marketing Research, 29(3), 329–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malmendier, U., & Tate, G. (2005). CEO overconfidence and corporate investment. Journal of Finance, 60(6), 2661–2700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mintzberg, H. (1973). The nature of managerial work. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. A., & Cain, D. M. (2007). Overconfidence and underconfidence: when and why people underestimate (and overestimate) the competition. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 197–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morwitz, V. G., & Schmittlein, D. C. (1998). Testing new direct marketing offerings: the interplay of management judgment and statistical models. Management Science, 44(5), 610–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • MouselabWEB (2012). Monitoring information acquisition processes on the Web, (accessed August 31, 2012), [available at http://www.mouselabweb.org/].

  • Myers, I. B., & McCaulley, M. H. (1985). Manual: a guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. A., & Bazerman, M. H. (1985). The effects of framing and negotiator overconfidence on bargaining behaviors and outcomes. Academy of Management Journal, 28(1), 34–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, B. R., & Shanks, D. R. (2003). Take the best or look at the rest? Factors influencing ‘one-reason’ decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(1), 53–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, B. R., Weston, N. J., & Shanks, D. R. (2003). Empirical tests of the fast-and-frugal heuristic: not everyone ‘takes-the-best’. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(1), 82–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nutt, P. C. (1984). Types of organizational decision processes. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29(3), 414–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, D. M. (2003). Not so fast! (and not so frugal!): rethinking the recognition heuristic. Cognition, 90(1), B1–B9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pan, W. (2001). Akaike’s information criterion in generalized estimating equations. Biometrics, 57(1), 120–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W. (1976). Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: an information search and protocol analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16(2), 366–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., & Bettman, J. R. (2004). Walking with the scarecrow: the information-processing approach to decision research. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 110–132). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 534–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Luce, M. F. (1996). When time is money: decision behavior under opportunity-cost time pressure. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(2), 131–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinartz, W. J., & Kumar, V. (2000). On the profitability of long-life customers in a noncontractual setting: an empirical investigation and implications for marketing. Journal of Marketing, 64(4), 17–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinartz, W. J., & Kumar, V. (2003). The impact of customer relationship characteristics on profitable lifetime duration. Journal of Marketing, 67(1), 77–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinartz, W. J., & Venkatesan, R. (2008). Models for customer relationship management (CRM). In B. Wierenga (Ed.), Handbook of marketing decision models (pp. 291–326). New York: Springer Science + Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinartz, W. J., Thomas, J. S., & Kumar, V. (2005). Balancing acquisition and retention resources to maximize customer profitability. Journal of Marketing, 69(1), 63–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuters (1996). Dying for information? An investigation into the effects of information overload in the UK and worldwide. London: Reuters Business Information.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieskamp, J., & Hoffrage, U. (1999). When do people use simple heuristics, and how can we tell. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, & the ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 141–167). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieskamp, J., & Hoffrage, U. (2008). Inferences under time pressure: how opportunity costs affect strategy selection. Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 258–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ronchetto, J. R., Hutt, M. D., & Reingen, P. H. (1989). Embedded influence pattern in organizational buying systems. Journal of Marketing, 53(4), 51–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruekert, R. W., & Walker, O. C., Jr. (1987). Marketing’s interaction with other functional units: a conceptual framework and empirical evidence. Journal of Marketing, 51(1), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russo, J. E., & Schoemaker, P. J. H. (1992). Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, 33(2), 7–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rust, R. T., Lemon, K. N., & Zeithaml, V. A. (2004). Return on marketing: using customer equity to focus marketing strategy. Journal of Marketing, 68(1), 109–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rust, R. T., Moorman, C., & Bhalla, G. (2010). Rethinking marketing. Harvard Business Review, 88(1/2), 94–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmittlein, D. C., & Peterson, R. A. (1994). Customer base analysis: an industrial purchase process application. Marketing Science, 13(1), 41–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmittlein, D. C., Morrison, D. G., & Colombo, R. (1987). Counting your customers: who are they and what will they do next? Management Science, 33(1), 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwenk, C. R. (1986). Information, cognitive biases, and commitment to a course of action. Academy of Management Review, 11(2), 298–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1981). The sciences of the artificial (2nd ed.). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41(February), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1997). Administrative behavior: a study of decision-making processes in administrative organizations (4th ed.). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M., & Houghton, S. M. (2003). The relationship between overconfidence and the introduction of risky products: evidence from a field study. Academy of Management Journal, 46(2), 139–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundström, G. A. (1987). Information search and decision making: the effects of information displays. Acta Psychologica, 65(2), 165–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1969). Intransitivity of preferences. Psychological Review, 76(1), 31–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: a theory of choice. Psychological Review, 79(4), 281–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgments under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Bruggen, G. H., & Wierenga, B. (2010). Marketing decision making and decision support: challenges and perspectives for successful marketing management support systems. Foundations and Trends® in Marketing, 4(4), 209–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbeke, G., & Molenberghs, G. (2000). Linear mixed models for longitudinal data (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoef, P. C., Reinartz, W. J., & Krafft, M. (2010). Customer engagement as a new perspective in customer management. Journal of Service Research, 13(3), 247–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K. (1987). Tacit knowledge in everyday intelligent behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1236–1247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wally, S., & Baum, J. R. (1994). Personal and structural determinants of the pace of strategic decision making. Academy of Management Journal, 37(4), 932–956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, J. P. (1995). Managerial and organizational cognition: notes from a trip down memory lane. Organizational Science, 6(3), 280–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2007). Linear mixed models: a practical guide using statistical software. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierenga, B., Van Bruggen, G. H., & Staelin, R. (1999). The success of marketing management support systems. Marketing Science, 18(3), 196–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., & Catty, S. (2006). Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychological Science, 17(9), 799–806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wübben, M., & Wangenheim, F. (2008). Instant customer base analysis - managerial heuristics often get it right. Journal of Marketing, 72(3), 82–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeger, S. L., & Liang, K.-Y. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics, 42(1), 121–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zorn, C. J. W. (2001). Generalized estimating equation models for correlated data: a review with applications. American Journal of Political Science, 45(2), 470–490.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Walter Herzog, Jan R. Landwehr, Bernd Skiera, and the four anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johannes C. Bauer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bauer, J.C., Schmitt, P., Morwitz, V.G. et al. Managerial decision making in customer management: adaptive, fast and frugal?. J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. 41, 436–455 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-012-0320-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-012-0320-7

Keywords

Navigation