Skip to main content
Log in

Explanation generation, not explanation expectancy, improves metacomprehension accuracy

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ability to monitor the status of one’s own understanding is important to accomplish academic tasks proficiently. Previous studies have shown that comprehension monitoring (metacomprehension accuracy) is generally poor, but improves when readers engage in activities that access valid cues reflecting their situation model (activities such as concept mapping or self-explaining). However, the question still remains as to which process, encoding or retrieving, causes the improvement of metacomprehension accuracy, and the findings of previous research on this matter have been inconsistent. This study examined whether college students’ metacomprehension accuracy improves when they expect, at the time of reading, that they will explain the content later (active encoding) or when they actually generate an explanation (encoding plus active retrieving). In the experiments, college students read five texts. During reading, some students expected that they would generate explanations but did not actually generate them. In contrast, some students actually generated an explanation of the text after reading. All students then rated their comprehension of each text. Finally, they completed tests on the materials. Results of both studies revealed that metacomprehension accuracy, operationalized as the association between comprehension ratings and test performance, was greater for the group that actually generated explanations than for the expectancy or control groups.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In both studies, graduate students were included as participants, but they were equally distributed between the three groups.

  2. One may argue that it is hard for participants to distinguish between what they knew before the experiment and what they learned in the experiment, and prior knowledge should be assessed at the start of the experiment. The study by Rozenblit and Keil (2002), however, demonstrated that college students could accurately evaluate the initial states of their knowledge even after reading the expert’s explanation. Rozenblit and Keil (2002) also showed that before reading experts’ explanations or answering test questions, participants overestimated their understanding of the device mechanism. This is why prior knowledge was assessed at the end of the experiment in our study.

  3. Some research argued that prior knowledge does not contribute to relative metacomprehension accuracy (Griffin et al. 2009). Note, however, that this does not mean that participants do not profit from prior knowledge of one or several texts to varying degrees when making judgments of their comprehension. For example, knowledge of baseball does not help readers improve relative metacomprehension accuracy when reading baseball texts. If, however, a set of materials consists of texts not only about baseball, but also about music, prior knowledge about baseball does contribute to accurate discrimination between well-learned materials (that is, baseball texts) and less well-learned materials (music texts) as suggested in Glenberg and Epstein (1987). In this context, we assume that prior knowledge might have positive impact when participants read texts.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (2009). Cognitive psychology and its implications (7th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C. M., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Why do delayed summaries improve metacomprehension accuracy? Acta Psychologica, 128, 110–118. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Annis, L. F. (1983). The processes and effects of peer tutoring. Human Learning, 2, 39–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Azevedo, R., Guthrie, J. T., & Seibert, D. (2004). The role of self-regulated learning in fostering students’ conceptual understanding of complex systems with hypermedia. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 30, 87–111. doi:10.2190/DVWX-GM1T-6THQ-5WC7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benware, C. A., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality of learning with an active versus passive motivational set. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 755–765. doi:10.3102/00028312021004755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bråten, I., & Strømsø, H. I. (2003). A longitudinal think-aloud study of spontaneous strategic processing during the reading of multiple expository texts. Reading and Writing, 16, 195–218. doi:10.1023/A:1022895207490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2000). Self-explaining expository texts: The dual processes of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology: Educational design and cognitive science (Vol. 5, pp. 161–238). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M. H., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1803_3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiang, E. S., Therriault, D. J., & Franks, B. A. (2010). Individual differences in relative metacomprehension accuracy: Variation within and across task manipulations. Metacognition and Learning, 5, 121–135. doi:10.1007/s11409-009-9052-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1969). The t test for means. In J. Cohen (Ed.), Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (pp. 17–71). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, E. B., Brown, A. L., & Rivkin, I. D. (1997). The effect of instructional explanations on learning from scientific texts. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6, 347–365. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0604_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crain-Thoreson, C., Lippman, M. Z., & McClendon-Magnuson, D. (1997). Windows on comprehension: Reading comprehension processes as revealed by two think-aloud procedures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 579–591. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.4.579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Bruin, A. B. H., Thiede, K. W., Camp, G., & Redford, J. (2011). Generating keywords improves metacomprehension and self-regulation in elementary and middle school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 294–310. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2005). Why does rereading improve metacomprehension accuracy? Evaluating the levels-of-disruption hypothesis for the rereading effect. Discourse Processes, 40, 37–55. doi:10.1207/s15326950dp4001_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (2009). The role of reader characteristics in processing and learning from informational text. Review of Educational Research, 79, 197–261. doi:10.3102/0034654308324654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fukaya, T. (2010). Factors affecting the accuracy of metacomprehension: A meta-analysis. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 236–251. doi:10.5926/jjep.58.236. In Japanese with English abstract.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Epstein, W. (1985). Calibration of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 702–718. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.1-4.702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Epstein, W. (1987). Inexpert calibration of comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 15, 84–93. doi:10.3758/BF03197714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Individual differences, rereading, and self-explanation: Concurrent processing and cue validity as constraints on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 36, 93–103. doi:10.3758/MC.36.1.93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, T. D., Jee, B. D., & Wiley, J. (2009). The effects of domain knowledge on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 37, 1001–1013. doi:10.3758/MC.37.7.1001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. (2009). The effects of explain-to-others and self-explanation as a learning strategy for understanding from a science expository text. The Science of Reading, 51, 107–118. In Japanese with English abstract.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itoh, M. (2004). Explanatory writing and constructive learning. Tokyo: Kazama Press. In Japanese.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kindersley, D. (1996). The Way Things Work 2.0. [CD-ROM]: DK Multimedia.

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., Welsch, D., Schmalhofer, F., & Zimny, S. (1990). Sentence memory: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 122–159. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(90)90069-C.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 126, 349–370. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, L., & Zabrucky, K. M. (1998). Calibration of comprehension: Research and implications for education and instruction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 345–391. doi:10.1006/ceps.1998.0972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macaulay, D. (1988). The way things work (H. Utasaki, Trans.). London: Dorling Kindersley Limited. (Japanese ed., 1990, Tokyo: Iwanami Press).

  • Maki, R. H. (1998). Test predictions over text material. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 117–144). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., & Berry, S. L. (1984). Metacomprehension of text material. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 663–679. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., & Swett, S. (1987). Metamemory for narrative text. Memory & Cognition, 15, 72–83. doi:10.3758/BF03197713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., Foley, J. M., Kajer, W. K., Thompson, R. C., & Willert, M. G. (1990). Increased processing enhances calibration of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 609–616. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., Jonas, D., & Kallod, M. (1994). The relationship between comprehension and metacomprehension ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 126–129. doi:10.3758/BF03200769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., Holder, E. W., & McGuire, M. J. (2001, November). Metacomprehension of text: A test of the optimum effort hypothesis. Paper presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Orlando, Florida.

  • Maki, R. H., Shields, M., Wheeler, A. E., & Zacchilli, T. L. (2005). Individual differences in absolute and relative metacomprehension accuracy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 723–731. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.4.723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., Willmon, C., & Pietan, A. (2009). Basis of metamemory judgments for text with multiple-choice, essay and recall tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 204–222. doi:10.1002/acp.1440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millis, K. L., Simon, S., & tenBroek, N. S. (1998). Resource allocation during the rereading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 26, 232–246. doi:10.3758/BF03201136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 109–133. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 26, pp. 125–173). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2000). The rereading effect: Metacomprehension accuracy improves across reading trials. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1004–1010. doi:10.3758/BF03209348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Stark, R., Gruber, H., & Mandl, H. (1998). Learning from worked-out examples: The effects of example variability and elicited self-explanations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 90–108. doi:10.1006/ceps.1997.0959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roy, M., & Chi, M. T. H. (2005). The self-explanation principle. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 271–286). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rozenblit, L., & Keil, F. (2002). The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science, 26, 521–562. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2605_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Metcalfe, J. (1994). Methodological problems and pitfalls in the study of human metacognition. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 93–113). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., & Anderson, M. C. M. (2003). Summarizing can improve metacomprehension accuracy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 129–160. doi:10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00011-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M. C. M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 66–73. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Dunlosky, J., Griffin, T. D., & Wiley, J. (2005). Understanding the delayed-keyword effect on metacomprehension accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1267–1280. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Redford, J. S. (2009). Metacognitive monitoring during and after reading. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 85–106). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Anderson, M. C. M. (2010). Poor metacomprehension accuracy as a result of inappropriate cue use. Discourse Processes, 47, 331–362. doi:10.1080/01638530902959927.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Wiley, J., & Griffin, T. D. (2011). Test expectancy affects metacomprehension accuracy. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 264–273. doi:10.1348/135910710X510494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007). The negative cascade of incongruent generative study-test processing in memory and metacomprehension. Memory & Cognition, 35, 668–678. doi:10.3758/BF03193305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Meter, P. (2001). Drawing construction as a strategy for learning from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 129–140. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von der Linden, N., Schneider, W., & Roebers, C. M. (2011). The effects of summary production and encoding condition on children’s metacognitive monitoring. Metacognition and Learning, 6, 3–23. doi:10.1007/s11409-010-9063-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, C. E., & Mayer, R. E. (1986). The teaching to learning strategies. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 315–327). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Griffin, T. D., & Thiede, K. W. (2005). Putting the comprehension in metacomprehension. The Journal of General Psychology, 132, 408–428. doi:10.3200/GENP.132.4.408-428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, Q., & Linderholm, T. (2008). Adult metacomprehension: Judgment processes and accuracy constraints. Educational Psychology Review, 20, 191–206. doi:10.1007/s10648-008-9073-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Appreciation is expressed to Shin-ichi Ichikawa for his helpful comments on an earlier version of this article. I also thank Toshihiko Endo, Mikiko Seo, Takanori Fukushi, Soichiro Iwata, Mika Morozumi, Hitoshi Nakajima, Yoshikazu Suzuki, Yuji Watanabe, Yuka Masamune, Arisa Morizawa, and Satoru Sugimoto for data collection, Tetsuya Kawamoto and Aiko Komoto for data analysis, and Emmanuel Manalo and Stephanie Coop for English proofreading. This research was partly supported in part by grant from Research Fellow of the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (23-9955).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tatsushi Fukaya.

Appendices

Appendix A. Example of text

Title: Toilet tank (Experiment 2)

When you press a toilet handle, you push water into a siphon pipe. Once the water flows through the pipe, air pressure causes all of the water in the tank flow rapidly through the tube into the bowl. The float drops with the water level, and water then flows into the tank. This makes the float rise, stopping the water flow.

figure a

Appendix B. Example of inference questions

Title: Toilet tank

(1) If you press a toilet handle, how does the disk move?

The disk goes up into the funnel. (Correct answer)

(2) Is there any problem if the disk is too small?

With a small disk, you can’t push enough water into the funnel, so the water never flows through a siphon pipe. (Correct answer)

(3) When the float drops with the water level, in what way does water flow into the tank?

The drop of the float opens the valve to the water inlet. (Correct answer)

(4) Why is there a float in a tank?

With the float, you can open the valve when the tank is empty, and close the valve when the tank is full. (Correct answer)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fukaya, T. Explanation generation, not explanation expectancy, improves metacomprehension accuracy. Metacognition Learning 8, 1–18 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-012-9093-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-012-9093-0

Keywords

Navigation