Skip to main content
Log in

The Danish Effect: Beginning to Explain High Well-Being in Denmark

  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although income and happiness have been linked at both the individual and national levels of analysis, few studies have specifically examined the different relationships between these two variables in affluent nations. This study investigates various measures of well-being in both the United States and Denmark. Respondents in both countries reported high levels of well-being but Americans generally reported greater positive and negative affect while Danes reported higher levels of satisfaction and enjoyment. Interestingly, low income respondents in the United States reported higher negative affect and lower life satisfaction than their counterparts in Denmark. For positive affect, the major difference between the two countries was found among high income respondents. The key to understanding differences in the well-being of these two nations appears to lie in understanding the well-being of the poor. Suggestions for future directions in research are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. First measures for the Danish sample was: χ 2 (33, N = 1004) = 88.27, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.04; and for the US sample: χ 2 (33, N = 1,001) = 99.56, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.05. To account for the dichotomous scoring of these items, we also ran the factor analysis with polychoric correlations, and got the following goodness of fit measures for the Danish sample: χ 2 (28, N = 1,004) = 56.25, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.04 and for the US sample: χ 2 (25, N = 1,001) = 45.19, p = 0.008, CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.03. The degrees of freedom cannot be unambiguously determined for a model with polychoric correlations, and the chi-square values cannot be used to test differences between models.

  2. The items were kept separate because the correlations between them were inconsistent with a one-factor model in both the American and the Danish sub-samples. This is because the product of the two highest correlations was higher than the lowest correlation in the correlation matrix, and such a matrix is not is compatible with the idea of a common factor (E. Røysamb, personal communication, 28. September 2008).

References

  • Aristotle (1986/4th Cent. BCE). Nichomachean ethics. New York: Macmillan.

  • Balatsky, G., & Diener, E. (1993). Subjective well-being among Russian students. Social Indicators Research, 28, 225–243. doi:10.1007/BF01079019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsson, X., & Kohler, X. (2001). Quality of life among children aged 2–17 years in the five Nordic countries: Comparison between 1984 and 1996. European Journal of Public Health, 11, 437–445. doi:10.1093/eurpub/11.4.437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Material wealth and subjective well-being. In M. Eid & R. Larsen (Eds.), The science of subjective well-being (pp. 307–322). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biswas-Diener, R., & Diener, E. (2006). Subjective well-being of the homeless, and related lessons for happiness. Social Indicators Research, 76, 185–205. doi:10.1007/s11205-005-8671-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biswas-Diener, R., Vittersø, J., & Diener, E. (2005). Most people are pretty happy, but there is cultural variation: The Inughuit, the Amish, and the Maasai. Journal of Happiness Studies, 6, 205–226. doi:10.1007/s10902-005-5683-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2001). Applying the rash model: Fundamental measurement in the human sciences. Mathwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinkerhoff, M. B., Fredell, K. A., & Frideres, J. S. (1997). Basic minimum needs, quality of life and selected correlates: Exploration in villages in northern India. Social Indicators Research, 42, 245–281. doi:10.1023/A:1006834830518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L. B. (1993). The psychology of religion in Scandinavia. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 3, 47–67. doi:10.1207/s15327582ijpr0301_7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantril, H. (1965). The pattern of human concern. Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, M. W. L., & Rensvold, R. B. (1999). Testing factorial invariance across groups: A reconseptualization and proposed new method. Journal of Management, 25, 1–27. doi:10.1177/014920639902500101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, K. A., Dorfman, L. T., & Tompkins, J. B. (1985). Life satisfaction of retired professors: The contribution of work, health, income, and length of retirement. Educational Gerontology, 11, 337–347. doi:10.1080/0380127850110602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being? A literature review and guide to needed research. Social Indicators Research, 57, 119–169. doi:10.1023/A:1014411319119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking the mysteries of psychological wealth. Boston, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1995). The wealth of nations revisited: Income and quality of life. Social Indicators Research, 36, 275–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Diener, M., & Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 851–864. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.851.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Horwitz, J., & Emmons, R. A. (1985). Happiness of the very wealthy. Social Indicators Research, 16, 263–274. doi:10.1007/BF00415126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Napa-Scollon, C. K., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., & Suh, E. M. (2000). Positivity and the construction of life satisfaction judgments: Global happiness is not the sum of its parts. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1, 159–176. doi:10.1023/A:1010031813405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Aroroa, R. (in press). Wanting, liking, and other reasons money might be associated with happiness: The First survey of Planet Earth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.

  • Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2000). Are Scandinavians happier than Asians? Issues in comparing nations on subjective well-being. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Politics and economics of Asia. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Suh, E. M. (1999). Measuring subjective well-being to compare quality of life of cultures. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being. Boston, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Economist. (2006). Pocket world in figures. London: Profile Books Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallup, Inc. (2007). The state of global well-being 2007. New York: Gallup Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haavind, H., & Magnusson, E. (2005). The Nordic countries–welfare paradises for women and children? Feminism & Psychology: An International Journal, 15, 229–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagerty, M. R. (2000). Social comparisons of income in one’s community: Evidence from national surveys of income and happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 764–771. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R., & Klingemann, H. D. (2000). Genes, culture, and happiness. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Subjective well-being across cultures. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasser, T. (2003). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • King James Bible. (1991). New York: Ivy Books.

  • Marx, C. (1848/1988). The communist manifesto. New York: WW Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, G., & Izuierdo, C. (2009). Anthropology, happiness and well-being. In G. Matthews & C. Izuierdo (Eds.), Pursuits of happiness: Well-being in anthropological perspective (1–19). New York: Berghahn Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2007). Mplus user’s guide (5th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rost, J., Carstensen, C., & von Davier, M. (1997). Applying the mixed rasch model to personality questionnaires. In J. Rost & R. Langeheine (Eds.), Application of latent trait and latent class models in the social sciences (pp. 324–332). Münster: Waxman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schor, J. B. (1999). The overspent American: Why we want what we don’t need. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schyns, P. (2000). The relationship between income, changes in income and life satisfaction in West Germany and the Russian Federation: Relative, absolute, or a combination of both? In E. Diener & D. R. Rahtz (Eds.), Advances in quality of life theory and research (Vol. 1, pp. 83–109). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R. (2000). Handbook of hope: Theory, measures, and applications. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tov, W., & Diener, E. (2008). The well-being of nations: Linking together trust, cooperation, and democracy. In B. A. Sullivan, M. Snyder, & J. L. Sullivan (Eds.), Cooperation: The political psychology of effective human interaction (pp. 323–342). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, J. (2008). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 242–259. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards an integrated analysis of bias in cross-cultural assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 13, 29–37. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.13.1.29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Social Indicators Research, 24, 1–34. doi:10.1007/BF00292648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1993). Happiness in nations: Subjective appreciation of life in 55 nations 1996–1990. Rotterdam: RISBO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R., & Hagerty, M. (2003). Rising happiness in nations 1946–2004: A reply to Easterlin. Social Indicators Research, 79, 421–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, J. (2002). Welfare production, poverty and wealth: A comparative and longitudinal perspective. In W. Glatzer (Ed.), Rich and poor: Disparities, perceptions, concomitants (pp. 235–270). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Davier, M. (2000). WINMIRA 32 user manual. Kiel, Germany: IPN—Institute for Science Education.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Biswas-Diener.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Biswas-Diener, R., Vittersø, J. & Diener, E. The Danish Effect: Beginning to Explain High Well-Being in Denmark. Soc Indic Res 97, 229–246 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9499-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9499-5

Keywords

Navigation