Skip to main content
Log in

Building literacy in alphabetic, abjad and morphosyllabic systems

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Preschoolers’ metalinguistic and visual capabilities may be associated with the writing system of their culture. We examined patterns of performance in phonological awareness, naming of letters, morphological awareness, and visual-spatial relations, in 5-year-old native speakers of Spanish (n = 43), Hebrew (n = 40), and Cantonese (n = 63) and the relations of these literacy related skills to concurrent word writing and word reading. The writing systems in these languages represent three major categories, i.e., alphabetic (Spanish), abjad (Hebrew), and morphosyllabic (Chinese). Phonological awareness, letter naming, and perception of visual-spatial relations differed across groups, whereas morphological awareness showed a similar level of attainment in all three languages. Stepwise regression analyses explaining writing and reading for each language separately revealed both commonalities and differences between languages. Phonological awareness assessed by initial syllable deletion contributed to writing and to reading in Cantonese and in Spanish. Phonological awareness assessed by final phoneme isolation explained reading in Hebrew, whereas final and initial phoneme isolation explained writing in Hebrew. Letter naming predicted both writing and reading in Spanish and in Hebrew, while perception of visual-spatial relations did so in Cantonese. At age 5, children’s metalinguistic knowledge and visual discrimination abilities are already attuned to the particular features of the writing system to which they are exposed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., & Taft, M. (2001). Syllables and morphemes: Contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 545–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alves Martins, M., & Silva, C. (2006). The impact of invented spelling on phonemic awareness. Learning and Instruction, 16, 41–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., Laudanna, A., & Romani, C. (1988). Lexical access and inflectional morphology. Cognition, 28, 297–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. G. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardoso-Martins, C. (1995). Sensitivity to rhymes, syllables, and phonemes in literacy acquisition in Portuguese. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 808–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Nomanbhoy, D. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., & Perea, M. (2002). Masked priming effects with syllabic neighbors in the lexical decision task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1228–1242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrillo, M. (1994). Development of phonological awareness and reading acquisition: A study in Spanish language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 279–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casalis, S., & Louis-Alexandre, M. F. (2000). Morphological analysis, phonological analysis and learning to read French: A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 303–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, L., & Louie, L. (1992). Developmental trend of Chinese preschool children in drawing and writing. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 93–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Hao, M., Geva, E., Zhu, J., & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in Chinese children’s vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 615–631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M. J., & Yuen, J. C. K. (1991). Effects of pinyin and script type on verbal processing: comparisons of China, Taiwan and Hong Kong experience. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 14, 429–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. K. H., & McBride-Chang, C. (in press). Executive functioning skills uniquely predict Chinese word reading. Journal of Educational Psychology.

  • Corcos, E. M., Kruk, R. S., & Willows, D. M. (1993). Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities. Makwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L., & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., & Suárez-Coalla, P. (2009). From grapheme to word in reading acquisition in Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 583–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, P. T., & Bright, W. (Eds.). (1996). The world’s writing systems. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Defior, S., & Tudela, P. (1994). Effect of phonological training on reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 299–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demont, E., & Gombert, J. E. (1996). Phonological awareness as a predictor of recoding skills and syntactic awareness as a predictor of comprehension skills. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 315–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dominguez, A. (1996). Evaluación de 1os efectos a largo plazo de la enseñanza de habilidades de análisis fonológico en el aprendizaje de la lectura y de la escritura [The evaluation of long range effects of the teaching of phonological analysis abilities on learning to read and to write]. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 76, 69–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, W. U. (1996). A functionalist semiotic model of morphonology. In R. Singh (Ed.), Trubetzkoy’s orphan (pp. 67–83). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foulin, J. A. (2005). Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 129–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, M. F. (1996). Test of visual-perceptual skills (non-motor). Burlingame, CA: Psychological and Educational Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E., & Levin, H. (1975). The psychology of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goikoetxea, E. (2005). Levels of phonological awareness in preliterate and literate Spanish-speaking children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 51–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2002). In the beginning was the rhyme? A reflection on Hulme, Hatcher, Nation, Brown, Adams, and Stuart. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 47–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. E. (1997). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S.-H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez, J. E., & Ortiz, M. R. (2000). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition in the Spanish language. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 3, 37–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I. (2007). The role of Hebrew letter names in early literacy: The case of multi-phonemic acrophonic names. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 193–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., & Ehri, L. C. (2009). Young children’s ability to read and spell their own and classmates’ names: The role of letter knowledge. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Ravid, D., & Rapaport, S. (2001). Morphology and spelling among Hebrew-speaking children: From kindergarten to first grade. Journal of Child Language, 28, 741–772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2005). Learning of letter names and sounds and contribution to word reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 139–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of early literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Growth and predictive relations of vocabulary and inflectional morphology in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 397–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mair, V. (1996). Modern Chinese writing. In P. T. Daniels & W. Bright (Eds.), The World’s writing systems (pp. 252–258). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, E., Chong, K., & Li, Y. (2004). Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 293–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., et al. (2005a). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological and morphological awareness to reading in Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and America. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W.-Y., Zhong, Y. -P., Burgess, S., & Hayward, W. (2005b). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 99–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Chung, K. K. H. & Tong, X. (2011a). Copying skills in relation to word reading and writing in Chinese children with and without dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(3), 422–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2000). Naming speed and phonological awareness in Chinese children: Relations to reading skills. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 1, 93–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C.-P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Zhou, Y., Cho, J.-R., Aram, D., Levin, I., & Tolchinsky, L. (2011b). Visual spatial skills: A consequence of learning to read? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 256–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 370–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicoladis, E. (2007). Preschool children’s acquisition of compounds. In G. Libben & G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 96–124). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Bindman, M. (1997). Morphological spelling strategies: Developmental stages and processes. Developmental Psychology, 33, 637–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G., & Sénéchal, M. (2008). Pathways to literacy: A study of invented spelling and its role in learning to read. Child Development, 79, 899–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Lefty, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puranik, C. S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). From scribbles to scrabble: Preschool children’s developing knowledge of written language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 567–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2005). Hebrew orthography and literacy. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 339–363). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D., & Malenky, D. (2001). Awareness of linear and nonlinear morphology in Hebrew: A developmental study. First Language, 21, 25–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read, C., Zhang, Y.-F., Nie, H.-Y., & Ding, B.-Q. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 31–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieben, L., Ntamakiliro, L., Gonthier, B., & Fayol, M. (2005). Effects of various early writing practices on reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 145–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rispens, J. E., McBride-Chang, C., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Morphological awareness and early and advanced reading and spelling in Dutch. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 587–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, J. (1993). La formación de palabras compuestas en inglés y en español [The formation of compound words in Spanish]. Revista de Filología Inglesa, 17, 21–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75–119). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S., Ehri, L. C., Olson, R. K., & Fowler, A. E. (1998). The fate of phonemic awareness beyond the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 115–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the anglocenticities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Blum, P. (2005). Syllable splitting in literate and pre-literate Hebrew speakers: Onsets and rimes or bodies and codas. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 182–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Gur, T. (1999). How reading begins: A study of preschoolers’ print identification strategies. Cognition and Instruction, 17, 177–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., MacLean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309–1324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (Cambridge Studies in Cognitive and Perceptual Development) (pp. 89–111). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

  • Shimron, J. (1993). The role of vowels in reading: A review of studies of English and Hebrew. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 52–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, A. (1996). Phonology. London, England: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sun, H. L., Sun, D. J., Huang, J. P., Li, D. J., & Xing, H. B. (1996). The description on the corpus system of modern studies. In Z. S. Luo & S. L. Yuan (Eds.), Studies of Chinese and Chinese character in the computer era (pp. 459–466). Tsinghua: Tsinghua University Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L. (2003). The cradle of culture and what children know about writing and numbers before being taught. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., Bigas, M., & Barragan, C. (2010). Pedagogical practices in the teaching of early literacy in Spain: Voices from the classroom and from the official curricula. Research Papers in Education, 25, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Teberosky, A. (1998). The development of word segmentation and writing in two scripts. Cognitive Development, 13, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky-Landsmann, L., & Levin, I. (1985). Writing in preschoolers: An age-related analysis. Applied Psycholinguistics, 6, 319–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 105–135). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2004). The case of case: Children’s knowledge and use of upper and lowercase letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 413–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1996). Letter names help children connect print and speech. Developmental Psychology, 32, 505–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 1524–1540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1990). Children’s sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes, and phonemes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 156–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A., & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 767–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. (2006). Foundations for studying basic processes in reading. In R. Malatesha Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 735–758). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodrome, S. E., & Johnson, K. E. (2009). The role of visual discrimination in the learning-to-read process. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 117–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Department of Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, as well as Hong Kong Government GRF grant 448608 for partially supporting the present study. Thanks are extended to Rotem Shapira and Sigal Shatil-Carmon from Tel Aviv University, and to Catalina Barragán from The University of Almeria. The paper was written while Liliana Tolchinsky enjoyed a grant from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (EDU2009-10321).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Liliana Tolchinsky.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tolchinsky, L., Levin, I., Aram, D. et al. Building literacy in alphabetic, abjad and morphosyllabic systems. Read Writ 25, 1573–1598 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9334-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9334-7

Keywords

Navigation