Skip to main content
Log in

Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The same neural structures involved in the unconscious modeling of our acting body in space also contribute to our awareness of the lived body and of the objects that the world contains. Neuroscientific research also shows that there are neural mechanisms mediating between the multi-level personal experience we entertain of our lived body, and the implicit certainties we simultaneously hold about others. Such personal and body-related experiential knowledge enables us to understand the actions performed by others, and to directly decode the emotions and sensations they experience. A common functional mechanism is at the basis of both body awareness and basic forms of social understanding: embodied simulation. It will be shown that the present proposal is consistent with some of the perspectives offered by phenomenology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R. 2002. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12(2): 169–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R. 2003. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.4(3): 165–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. 2003. Dissociable neural systems for recognizing emotions. Brain Cogn. 52(1): 61–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G. and Damasio, A. R. 2000. A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J. Neurosci. 20: 2683–2690.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, A. and Frassinetti, F. 2000. When far becomes near: Re-mapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12: 415–420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, A. and Rizzolatti, G. 1992. Visual processing without awareness: Evidence from unilateral neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4: 345–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, C., Desimone, R. and Gross, C. G. 1981. Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. J. Neurophysiol. 46: 369–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G. and Freund, H.-J. 2001.Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience 13: 400–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buccino, G., Lui, F., Canessa, N., Patteri, I., Lagravinese, G., Benuzzi, F., Porro, C. A. and Rizzolatti, G. 2004. Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: An fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16: 114–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N. and Young, A. W. 2000. Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nat. Neurosci. 3: 1077–1078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C. and Lenzi, G. L. 2003. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100(9): 5497–5502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cochin, S., Barthelemy, C., Lejeune, B., Roux, S. and Martineau, J. 1998. Perception of motion and qEEG activity in human adults. Electroenc. Clin. Neurophysiol. 107: 287–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowey, A., Small, M. and Ellis, S. 1994. Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than near space. Neuropsychologia 32: 1059–1066.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cowey, A., Small, M. and Ellis, S. 1999. No abrupt change in visual hemineglect from near to far space. Neuropsychologia 37: 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J. and Gre‘zes, J. 1999. Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 172–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., Grezes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F. and Fazio, F. 1997. Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject’s strategy.Brain 120: 1763–1777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duhamel, J-R., Bremmer, F., Ben Hamed, S. and Graf, W. 1997. Spatial invariance of visual receptive fields in parietal cortex neurons. Nature 389: 845–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson, H. H., Geyer, S. and Naito, E. 2003. Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. J. Neurophysiol. 90(5): 3304–3316.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G. and Rizzolatti, G. 1995. Motor facilitation during action observation: A magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73: 2608–2611.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G. and Fogassi, L. 2003. Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 17: 1703–1714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Fadiga, L. and Rizzolatti, G. 1996a. Space coding in inferior premotor cortex (area F4): Facts and speculations. In: F.Laquaniti and P. Viviani (eds.), Neural Basis of Motor Behavior (pp. 99–120).NATO ASI Series, Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Luppino, G., Matelli, M. and Rizzolatti, G.1996b. Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex (area F4). J. Neurophysiol. 76: 141–157.

  • Gallese, V. 2000. The acting subject: Towards the neural basis of social cognition. In: T.Metzinger (ed.), Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Empirical and Conceptual Questions (pp. 325–333).Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2001. The “Shared Manifold” Hypothesis: From mirror neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8(5–7): 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2003a. The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: The quest for a common mechanism. Phil. Trans. Royal Soc. London B. 358: 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2003b. The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 36(4): 171–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2003c. A neuroscientific grasp of concepts: From control to representation. Phil. Trans. Royal Soc. London, B. 358: 1231–1240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2005. “Being like me”: Self-other identity, mirror neurons and empathy. In :S. Hurley and N. Chater (eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2004. Intentional attunement. The mirror neuron system and its role in inter- personal relations. http://www.interdisciplines.org/mirror/papers/1

  • Gallese, V. and Goldman, A. 1998. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. and Lakoff, G. 2005. The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cogn. Neuropsychol (in press).

  • Gallese, V. and Metzinger, T. 2003. Motor ontology: The representational reality of goals, actions, and selves. Philosophical Psychology 13(3): 365–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. and Rizzolatti, G. 1996. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119: 593–609.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C. and Rizzolatti, G. 2004. A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 396–403.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L. and Rizzolatti, G. 2002b. Action Representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In: W. Prinz and B. Hommel (eds.), Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 247–266). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., Scandolara, C., Pigarev, I. N. and Rizzolatti, G. 1983. Visual responses in the postarcuate cortex (area 6) of the monkey that are independent of eye position. Experimental Brain Research 50: 464–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., Fogassi, L., Luppino, G., Matelli, M., Camarda, R. and Rizzolatti, G. 1988. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey: I.Somatotopy and the control of proximal movements. Experimental Brain Research 71: 475–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 1989. Interpretation psychologized. Mind and Language 4: 161–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 1992a. In defense of the simulation theory. Mind and Language 7: 104–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 1992b. Empathy, mind, and morals: Presidential address.Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association 66: 17–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 1993a. The psychology of folk psychology. Behavioral Brain Sciences 16: 15–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 1993b. Philosophical Applications of Cognitive Science. Boulder, Colo., Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 2000. The mentalizing folk. In: D. Sperber (ed.), Metarepresentation, London, Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. and Gallese, V. 2000. Reply to Schulkin. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 255–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. and Sripada, C. S. 2005. Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition (in press).

  • Goodale, M. A. and Milner, A. D. 1992. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15: 20–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. 1986. Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language 1: 158–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. 1995. Simulation without introspection or inference from me to you. In: M. Davies and T. Stone (eds.), Mental Simulation (pp. 53–67). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. 1996. ‘Radical’ simulationism. In: P. Carruthers and P. Smith (eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind (pp. 11–21). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. 2005. Intentional agents like myself. In: S. Hurley and N. Chater (eds.), Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafton, S. T., Arbib, M. A., Fadiga, L. and Rizzolatti, G. 1996.Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Exp. Brain Res. 112: 103–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, M. S. A. and Gross, C. G. 1995. The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal visual-tactile neurons. In: M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 1021–1034). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, M. S. A., Hu, X. and Gross, C. G. 1997a. Visuo-spatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 77: 2268–2292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, M. S. A., Hu, X. and Gross, C. G. 1997b. Coding the locations of objects in the dark. Science 277: 239–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, M. S. A., Reiss, L. A. J. and Gross, C. G. 1999. A neuronal representation of the location of nearby sounds. Nature 397: 428–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, M. S. A., Yap, G. S. and Gross, C. G. 1994. Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 266: 1054–1057.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan, P. W. and Marshall, J. C. 1991. Left neglect for near but not far space in man. Nature 350: 498–500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hari, R., Forss, N., Avikainen, S., Kirveskari, S., Salenius, S. and Rizzolatti, G. 1998. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: A neuromagnetic study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95: 15061–15065.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. 1925/1962. Phänomenologische Psychologie (1925) (Husserliana, vol. IX), Martinus Nijhoff, Den Haag.

  • Husserl, E. 1989. Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy Second Book: Studies in the Phenomenology of Constitution. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C. and Rizzolatti, G. 1999. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286: 2526–2528.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, P. and Jeannerod, M. 2003. Ways of Seeing – The Scope and Limits of Visual Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keysers, C., Kohler, E., Umilta‘, M. A., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G. and Gallese, V. 2003. Audio-visual mirror neurones and action recognition.Exp. Brain Res. 153: 628–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keysers, C., Wickers, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V. 2004. A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron 42(22): 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta‘, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G. 2002. Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297: 846–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krolak-Salmon, P., Henaff, M. A., Isnard, J., Tallon-Baudry, C., Guenot, M., Vighetto, A., Bertrand, O. and Mauguiere, F. 2003. An attention modulated response to disgust in human ventral anterior insula. Ann. Neurol. 53: 446–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambie, J. A. and Marcel, A. J. 2002. Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychol. Rev. 109: 219–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. F. and Halligan, P. W. 1988. Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature 336: 766–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metzinger, T. and Gallese, V. 2003. The emergence of a shared action ontology: Building blocks for a theory. Consciousness and Cognition 12: 549–571.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W. and Faulk, M. E. 1955. The insula: Further observations on its function. Brain 78: 445–470.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Millikan, R. G. 2004. Varieties of Meaning. Boston, MA: MIT Press, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., Rolls, E. T. and Caan, W. 1982. Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 47: 329–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Berti, A. and Gallese, V. 2000. Spatial neglect: Neurophysiological bases, cortical circuits and theories. In: F. Boller, J.Grafman, and G. Rizzolatti (eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology 2nd Edition (Vol. I), Amsterdam; Elsevier Science B.V., pp. 503–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Matelli, M. and Pavesi, G. 1983. Deficits in attention and movement following the removal of postarcuate (area 6) and prearcuate (area 8) cortex in macaque monkeys. Brain 106: 655–673.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V. 2000. Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping and action recognition: A new view on the cortical motor functions. In: M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 539–552), 2nd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V. 2001. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Neuroscience Reviews, 2: 661–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. and Gallese, V. 2002. Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex. Curr. Op. Neurobiol. 12: 149–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V. and Fogassi, L. 1996a. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cog. Brain Res. 3: 131–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D. and Fazio, G. 1996b. Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Exp. Brain Res. 111: 246–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. 1992. The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge, MA: London, England: MIT Press, A Bradford Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umilta‘, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C. and Rizzolatti, G. 2001. “I know what you are doing”: A neurophysiological study. Neuron 32: 91–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. 2002. First-person thoughts and embodied self-awareness: Some reflections on the relation between recent analytical philosophy and phenomenology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 1: 7–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, K. E., Strafella, A. P. and Paus, T. 2003. Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia 41(8): 989–994.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V. and Rizzolatti, G. 2003. Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40: 655–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vittorio Gallese.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gallese, V. Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience. Phenom Cogn Sci 4, 23–48 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-005-4737-z

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-005-4737-z

Navigation