Skip to main content
Log in

Committing to implementation intentions: Attention and memory effects for selected situational cues

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies tested whether forming implementation intentions (Gollwitzer, Am Psychol 54:493–503 in 1999) results in a heightened activation of specified situational cues. Going beyond prior studies, participants of the present studies specified these opportunities on their own (i.e., the action cues were not assigned by the experimenter), and activation level was assessed by attraction of attention and recall performance rather than lexical decisions. In Study 1, situational cues associated with the where and when to act on an everyday life goal attracted more attention than non-specified cues when presented to the non-attended channel in a dichotic listening task. In Study 2, the recall of specified cues was better than that of non-specified cues both 15 min after forming implementation intentions and after a delay of 2 days. Importantly, goal commitment and implementation intention commitment moderated this effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Motivation and volition in the course of action. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Implementation intentions and shielding goal striving from unwanted thoughts and feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 381–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badets, A., Blandin, Y., & Bouquet, C. A. (2006). The intention superiority effect in motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 491–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. (2009). Responding to subliminal cues: Do if-then plans cause action preparation and initiation without conscious intent? Social Cognition, 27, 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, U. C., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Achtziger, A. (2010). Staying on track: Planned goal striving is protected from disruptive internal states. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 505–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1954). The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 191–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryden, M. P. (1988). An overview of the dichotic listening procedure and its relation to cerebral organization. In K. Hughdahl (Ed.), Handbook of dichotic listening: Theory, methods, and research (pp. 1–43). Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, E. C., & Taylor, W. K. (1954). Some further experiments upon the recognition of speech with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 554–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A.-L., Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Self-regulatory strategy and executive control: Implementation intentions modulate task switching and Simon task performance. Psychological Research, 72, 12–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1982). Electrodermal responses to attended and nonattended significant stimuli during dichotic listening. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 315–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockree, P. M., & Ellis, J. A. (2001). Forming and cancelling everyday intentions: Implications for prospective remembering. Memory & Cognition, 29, 1139–1145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Düker, H. (1953). Konzentrations-Leistungs-Test K-L-T. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, B. A., & Erickson, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 220–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gadea, M., Espert, R., & Chirivella, J. (1997). Dichotic listening: Elimination of the right ear advantage under dual task procedure. Applied Neuropsychology, 4, 117–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., Gawrilow, C., & Oettingen, G. (2010). The power of planning: Self-control by effective goal-striving. In R. R. Hassin, K. N. Ochsner, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Self control in society, mind, and brain (pp. 279–296). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (1993). Representation of intentions: Persisting activation in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1211–1226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, J., Marsh, R. L., & Russel, E. J. (2000). The properties of retention intervals and their affect on retaining prospective memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1160–1169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kazén, M., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2008). Individual differences in intention initiation under demanding conditions: Interactive effects of state vs. action orientation and enactment difficulty. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 693–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ben-Zur, H., & Nussbaum, A. (1990). Encoding information for future action: Memory for to-be-performed tasks versus memory for to-be-recalled tasks. Memory & Cognition, 18, 568–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994). State orientation and the activation and retrieval of intentions from memory. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 127–154). Göttingen/Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, J. L. (1970). Semantic processing of unattended messages using dichotic listening. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 225–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, R. L., Cook, G. I., Meeks, J. T., Clark-Foos, A., & Hicks, J. L. (2007). Memory for intention-related material presented in a to-be-ignored channel. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1197–1204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Bink, M. L. (1998). Activation of completed, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 350–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Bryan, E. S. (1999). The activation of unrelated and cancelled intentions. Memory & Cognition, 27, 320–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maylor, E. A., Darby, R. J., & Della Sala, S. (2000). Retrieval of performed versus to-be-performed tasks: A naturalistic study of the intention-superiority effect in normal aging and dementia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 83–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, G. B., Li, P., & Kirk, E. R. (2004). The implicit volition model: On the preconscious regulation of temporarily adopted goals. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 317–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielson, S. L., & Sarason, I. G. (1981). Emotion, personality and selective attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 945–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by implementation intentions: Effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penningroth, S. (2005). Free recall of everyday retrospective and prospective memories: The intention-superiority effect is moderated by action versus state orientation and by gender. Memory, 13, 711–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruoff, A. (1981). Häufigkeitswörterbuch gesprochener Sprache. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sætrevik, B., & Hugdahl, K. (2007). Endogenous and exogenous control of attention in dichotic listening. Neuropsychology, 21, 285–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intention and repeated behaviour: Augmenting the predictive validity of the theory of planned behaviour. European Journal of Psychology, 29, 349–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goal intentions and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. E. (2010). What costs do reveal, moving beyond the cost debate: Reply to Einstein, McDaniel. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 756–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spranger, T., Schatz, T. R., & Knopf, M. (2008). Does action make you faster? A retrieval-based approach to investigating the origins of the enactment effect. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 487–495.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. (1974). Semantic processing in dichotic listening? A replication. Memory & Cognition, 2, 641–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., Moors, A., van Damme, S., & Crombez, G. (2010). The automatic orienting of attention to goal-relevant stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 134, 61–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Identifying good opportunities to act: Implementation intentions and cue discrimination. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 407–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 132, 249–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implementation intention effects: The role of goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 373–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wieber, F., & Sassenberg, K. (2006). I can’t take my eyes off of it: Attention attraction effects of implementation intentions. Social Cognition, 24(6), 723–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wixted, J. (1992). Subjective memorability and the mirror effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(4), 681–690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anja Achtziger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Achtziger, A., Bayer, U.C. & Gollwitzer, P.M. Committing to implementation intentions: Attention and memory effects for selected situational cues. Motiv Emot 36, 287–300 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9261-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9261-6

Keywords

Navigation