Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Olfaction, Emotion and Associative Learning: Effects on Motivated Behavior

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted to investigate emotional associative learning to odors and subsequent behavioral effects. In Experiment 1, participants experienced a frustration mood induction in the presence of an unfamiliar ambient odor and later worked on puzzle tests in a room scented with either the same-odor, a different-odor, or no-odor. Participants in the same-odor condition spent significantly less time working on the tests than participants in the other conditions; however, test accuracy did not vary. To clarify the findings, Experiment 2 included a test-only control and an emotionally neutral same-odor conditions. Results were compatible with the conclusion that decreased time spent by participants in the negative-same-odor condition was due to emotions elicited by associative learning to the ambient odor, although alternative interpretations remain possible. These data extend our previous results with children and suggest that odors readily become associated to emotions and can thereby influence behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alaoui-Ismaili, O., Robin, O., Rada, H., Dittmar, A., & Vernet-Maury, E. (1997). Basic emotions evoked by odorants: Comparison between autonomic responses and self-evaluation. Physiology & Behavior, 62(4), 713–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Schicker, I., Laska, M., Hudson, R., Distel, H., Kobayakawa, T., & Saito, S. (1998). Differences in perception of everyday odors: A Japanese-German cross-cultural study. Chemical Senses, 23(1), 31–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., van den Bergh, O., & Crombez, G. (1990). Flavor–flavor and color–flavor conditioning in humans. Learning and Motivation, 21, 434–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baeyens, F., & Wrzesniewksi, A., De Houwer, J., & Eelen, P. (1996). Toilet rooms, body massages, and smells: Two field studies on human evaluative odor conditioning. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 15, 77–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balogh, R. D., & Porter, R. H. (1986). Olfactory preferences resulting from mere exposure in human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(4), 395–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, P., & Wysocki, C. J. (1996). The nature and duration of adaptation following long-term odor exposure. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(5), 781–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, L. B., & Porter, R. H. (1991). Persistent effects of early odor exposure on human neonates. Chemical Senses, 16(2), 169–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dilks, D., Dalton, P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1999). Cross-cultural variation in responses to malodors. Chemical Senses, 24, 599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlichman, H., & Bastone, L. (1992). The use of odour in the study of emotion. In S. van Toller, & G. H. Dodd (Eds.), Fragrance—The psychology and biology of perfume (pp. 143–159). London: Elsevier Applied Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlichman, H., Kuhl, S. B., Zhu, J., & Warrenburg, S. (1997). Startle reflex modulation by pleasant and unpleasant odors in a between-subjects design. Psychophysiology, 34(6), 726–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engen, T. (1988). The acquisition of odour hedonics. In S. van Toller, & G. H. Dodd (Eds.), Perfumery—The psychology and biology of fragrance (pp. 79–90). London: Chapman and Hall Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engen, T. (1991). Odor sensation and memory. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epple, G., & Herz, R. S. (1999). Ambient odors associated to failure influence cognitive performance in children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35(2), 103–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feather, N. T. (1961). The relationship of persistence at a task to expectation of success and achievement related motives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), 552–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: Affective influences on interpersonal behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herz, R. S. (1997a). The effects of cue distinctiveness on odor-based context-dependent memory. Memory & Cognition, 25(3), 375–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herz, R. S. (1997b). Emotion experienced during encoding enhances odor retrieval cue effectiveness. American Journal of Psychology, 110(4), 489–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herz, R. S. (1999). Caffeine effects on mood and memory. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 37(9), 869–879.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herz, R. S. (2001). Ah, sweet skunk: Why we like or dislike what we smell. Cerebrum, 3(4), 31–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herz, R. S., Beland, S. L., & Hellerstein, M. (2004). Changing odor hedonic perception through emotional associations in humans. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 17, 315–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., Levine, G. M., McDonald, H. E., Melton, J., & Martin, L. L. (1997). The role of mood in quantitative and qualitative aspect of performance: Single or multiple mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 602–629.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 179–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.). Handbook of Emotions, 2nd Edition (pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A., Rosenzweig, A. S., & Young, M. J. (1991). The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making, 11, 221–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kavanaugh, J. K. (1987). The content of implicit leadership theories: An investigation of achievement orientation, task condition and relationship orientation under varied task difficulty and achievement conditions. Dissertation Abstracts International, 47(9-A), 3438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirk-Smith, M. D., Van Toller, C., & Dodd, G. H. (1983). Unconscious odour conditioning in human subjects. Biological Psychology, 17(2–3), 221–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knasko, S. C. (1992). Ambient odor’s effect on creativity, mood, and perceived health. Chemical Senses, 17(1), 27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knasko, S. C. (1995). Pleasant odors and congruency: Effects on approach behavior. Chemical Senses, 20(5), 479–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond, P. F., Davis, N. R., & O’Flaherty, A. S. (2000). Protection from extinction in human fear conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 967–983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lott, I., Sullivan, M., & McPherson, D. (1989). Associative olfactory learning occurs in the neonate. Neurology, 39, 110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & Levey, A. B. (1978). Evaluative conditioning. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 1, 57–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L. L., Achee, J. W., Ward, D. W., & Harlow, T. F. (1993). The role of cognition and effort in the use of emotions to guide behavior. In R. S. Wyer, & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 6, pp. 147–157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennella, J. A. (1995). Mother’s milk: A medium for early flavor experiences. Journal of Human Lactation, 11(1), 39–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1991). The transfer of alcohol to human milk. Effects on flavor and the infant’s behavior. New England Journal of Medicine, 325(14), 981–985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1993). The effects of repeated exposure to garlic-flavored milk on the nursling’s behavior. Pediatrics Research, 34(6), 805–808.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miltner, W., Matjak, M., Braun, C., Diekmann, H., & Brody, S. (1994). Emotional qualities of odors and their influence on the startle reflex in humans. Psychophysiology, 31(1), 107–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Otto, T., Cousens, G., & Herzog, C. (2000). Behavioral and neuropsychological foundations of olfactory fear conditioning. Behavioral Brain Research, 110, 119–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robin, O., Alaoui-Ismaili, O., Dittmar, A., & Vernet-Mauri, E. (1998). Emotional responses evoked by dental odors: An evaluation from autonomic parameters. Journal of Dental Research, 77, 1638–1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotton, J. (1983). Affective and cognitive consequences of malodorous pollution. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 4(2), 171–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russ, S. W. (1993). Affect and creativity: The role of affect and play in the creative process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (1999). On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 3–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelsohn, G. (1989). Affect Grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffman, S. S., Sattely-Miller, E. A., Suggs, M. S., & Graham, B. G. (1995). The effect of pleasant odors and hormone status on mood of women at midlife. Brain Research Bulletin, 36(1), 19–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffman, S. S., Suggs, M. S., & Sattely-Miller, E. A. (1995). Effect of pleasant odors on mood of males at midlife: Comparison of African-American and European-American men. Brain Research Bulletin, 36(1), 31–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schleidt, M., Hold, B., & Attili, G. (1981). A cross-cultural study on the attitude towards personal odors. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 7, 19–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, M. J., & Conway, M. (1989). Negative affect leads to low-effort cognition: Attributional processing for observed social behavior. Social Cognition, 7(4), 315–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, R. M., & Toubas, P. (1998). Clinical usefulness of maternal odor in newborns: Soothing and feeding preparatory responses. Biology of the Neonate, 74, 402–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vermetten, E., & Bremner, J. D. (2003). Olfaction as a traumatic reminder in posttraumatic stress disorder: Case reports and review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(2), 202–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, E. A., & Miller, R. R. (1997). What’s elementary about associative learning? Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 573–607.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rachel S. Herz Ph.D..

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Herz, R.S., Schankler, C. & Beland, S. Olfaction, Emotion and Associative Learning: Effects on Motivated Behavior. Motiv Emot 28, 363–383 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-004-2389-x

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-004-2389-x

Keywords

Navigation