Skip to main content
Log in

Brief Report: Perceptual Load and the Autism Spectrum in Typically Developed Individuals

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A fundamental task of the cognitive system is to prioritize behaviourally relevant sensory inputs for processing at the expense of irrelevant inputs. In a study of neurotypical participants (n = 179), we utilized a brief flanker interference task while varying the perceptual load of the visual display. Typically, increasing perceptual load (i.e., with greater numbers of search items) reduces interference from a competing peripheral distractor. We show that individuals who score above average on the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) show stronger interference at high perceptual load than individuals with below-average AQ scores. This is consistent with recent findings in individuals with autism spectrum conditions, and supports the idea that the cognitive style of the autistic brain is reflected in a broader phenotype across the population.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Notes

  1. Data from an additional 11 participants were discarded due to high error rates (>30%).

  2. The pattern of results is the same with the cut-offs originally used by Bayliss and Tipper (2005). With Low (AQ < 14; n = 82) and High (AQ > 17; n = 53) groups showing equal interference effects at Set Size Two, t(133) = 1.04, p = .30, d = .18, but the High AQ group showed stronger interference at Set Size Four, t(133) = 2.14, p = .035, d = .38. Finally, we also calculated linear correlations between AQ score and RT Interference (in ms) for both Set Sizes for the sample as a whole. At Set Size Two, there was no significant linear relationship, r = .09, n = 179, p = .25, two-tailed. Corroborating the other analyses, the correlation was significant at Set Size Four, r = .17, n = 179, p = .027, two-tailed. The quadratic relationship between AQ score and interference effect magnitude was non-significant (r < .1).

References

  • Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2010). A new step towards understanding Embedded Figures Test performance in the autism spectrum: The radial frequency search task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 374–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 248–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001b). The Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 631–650.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism spectrum as a function of target context. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 95–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2006). Predictive gaze cues and personality judgments: Should eye trust you? Psychological Science, 17(6), 514–520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48(3), 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Webb, S., Schellenberg, G. D., Dager, S., Friedman, S., Aylward, E., et al. (2002). Defining the broader phenotype of autism: genetic, brain, and behavioral perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 14(3), 581–611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B., & Eriksen, C. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E. (1999). The epidemiology of autism: A review. Psychological Medicine, 29, 769–786.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinter, E. J., Maybery, M. T., Van Beek, P. L., Pellicano, E., Badcock, J. C., & Badcock, D. R. (2009a). Global visual processing and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(9), 1278–1290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grinter, E. J., Van Beek, P. L., Maybery, M. T., & Badcock, D. R. (2009b). Visuospatial analysis and self-rated autistic-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(4), 670–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 216–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218–1220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, E., van Wingen, G., Bos, P., Putman, P., & van Honk, J. (2009). Reduced spontaneous facial mimicry in women with autistic traits. Biological Psychology, 80(3), 348–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 75–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in “theory of mind”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 528–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maylor, E. A., & Lavie, N. (1998). The influence of perceptual load on age differences in selective attention. Psychology and Aging, 13(4), 563–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(5), 777–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puzzo, I., Cooper, N. R., Vetter, P., Russo, R., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2009). Reduced cortico-motor facilitation in a normal sample with high traits of autism. Neuroscience Letters, 467(2), 173–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Swettenham, J., Campbell, R., & Coleman, M. (2009). Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1388–1393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49(22), 2705–2739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Merryn Constable, Dana Schneider, Sheyna Ezrapour, Harriet Dempsey-Jones, Brenda Ocampo, Elizabeth Beadle, Jasmine Loo and Daphne Bryan for assistance with data collection. This work was supported by a UQ Postdoctoral Fellowship awarded to Andrew Bayliss.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew P. Bayliss.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bayliss, A.P., Kritikos, A. Brief Report: Perceptual Load and the Autism Spectrum in Typically Developed Individuals. J Autism Dev Disord 41, 1573–1578 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1159-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1159-8

Keywords

Navigation