Skip to main content
Log in

Inhibitory Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Impairments in executive abilities such as cognitive flexibility have been identified in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). It remains unclear, however, whether such individuals also experience impairments in another executive ability: inhibitory control. In the present study, we administered three inhibitory tasks to 18 children with ASD, 23 siblings of children with ASD, and 25 typically developing children. After controlling for individual differences in age, overall IQ, and processing speed, children with ASD demonstrated impaired performance on two of the three inhibitory tasks. Results suggest that children with ASD experience circumscribed deficits in some but not all aspects of inhibitory control. More generally, the findings underscore the importance of using multiple measures to assess a putative single cognitive ability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Eskes et al. (1990) interpreted the results of their study in terms of the children’s ability to comprehend word meanings and not as evidence of intact inhibitory control per se.

References

  • Alexander, G., DeLong, M., & Strick, P. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 357–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asarnow, R., Brown, W., & Strandburg, R. (1995). Children with schizophrenic disorder: Neurobehavioral studies. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience, 245, 70–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, W., Zencius, A., Wesolowski, M., & Doubleday, F. (1991). Improving executive function disorders in brain-injured clients. Brain Injury, 5, 241–252.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, S. (2003). Executive function in context: Development, measurement, theory, and experience. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 138–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, C., Robertson, L., & Nordahl, T. (1992). Abnormal processing of irrelevant information in schizophrenia: Selective enhancement of Stroop facilitation. Psychiatry Research, 41, 137–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B., Castellanos, F., Giedd, J., Marsh, W., Hamburger, S., Schubert, A., et al. (1997). Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 374–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B., Durston, S., & Fossella, J. (2001). Evidence for a mechanistic model of cognitive control. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 1, 267–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B., Tottenham, N., & Fossella, J. (2002). Clinical, imaging, lesion, and genetic approaches toward a model of cognitive control. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 237–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christ, S., Steiner, R., Grange, D., Abrams, R., & White, D. (in press). Inhibitory control in children with prefrontal dysfunction related to phenylketonuria. Developmental Neuropsychology.

  • Christ, S., White, D., Brunstrom, J., & Abrams, R. (2003). Inhibitory control following prenatal brain injury. Neuropsychology, 17, 171–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christ, S., White, D., Mandernach, T., & Keys, B. (2001). Inhibitory control across the life-span. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 653–669.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chugani, D., Muzik, O., Rothermel, R., Behen, M., Chakraborty, P., Mangner, T., et al. (1997). Altered serotonin synthesis in the dentatothalamocortical pathway in autistic boys. Annals of Neurology, 42, 666–669.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Comalli, P., Wapner, W., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 47–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, J. (1995). Anatomic and behavioral aspects of frontal-subcortical circuits. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 1–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dagenbach D., & Carr T. (Eds.). (1994). Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, & language. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Ciaramitaro, V., Donner, E., Djali, S., & Robinson, M. (1994). An animal model of early-treated PKU. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 3072–3082.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Prevor, M., Callendar, G., & Druin, D. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62, 1–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drewe, E. (1975). Go–nogo learning after frontal lobe lesions in humans. Cortex, 11, 8–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J., & Akhtar, N. (1989). A developmental study of filtering in visual attention. Child Development, 60, 1188–1199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. (1995). The flankers task and response competition: A useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems. Visual Cognition, 2, 101–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B., & Eriksen, C. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eskes, G., Bryson, S., & McCormick, T. (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 20, 61–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geurts, H., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 45, 836–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golden, C. (1978). Stroop color and word test. Chicago: Stoelting.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, E., Pennington, B., Wehner, E., & Rogers, S. (1999). Executive function in young children in autism. Child Development, 70, 817–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, W. (1994). Statistics. (5th ed.) Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. (2004a). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. (2004b). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 26–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Plumet, M., & Leboyer, M. (1999). Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: Increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 40, 705–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34, 257–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Insel, T. (1988). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: A neuroethological perspective. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 365–369.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 34, 81–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi, S., Kawazu, M., Uchida, I., Kikyo, H., Asakura, I., & Miyashita, Y. (1999). Contribution of working memory to transient activation in human inferior prefrontal cortex during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 745–753.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leckman, J., Price, R., Walkup, J., Ort, S., Pauls, D., & Cohen, J. (1987). Nongenetic factors in Gilles de la Tourette’s syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, H., Culhane, K., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., Mattson, A., Harward, H., et al. (1991). Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 377–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A user’s guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach, & T. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Minshew, N., Garver, K., Lazar, N., Thulborn, K., Eddy, W., et al. (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology, 59, 834–840.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller B., & Cummings J. (Eds.). (1999). The human frontal lobes. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B., & Petrides, M. (1984). Behavioural effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. Trends in Neurosciences, 7, 403–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minshew, N., Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. (1999). Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but no cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology, 52, 917–922.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Hashimoto, T., Kunihiro, T., Nishikawa, M., Uema, T., et al. (2000). Abnormal regional blood flow in childhood autism. Brain, 123, 1838–1844.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. (1996). Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: A biologically based model of child psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 19–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S., Farnham, J., & Pennington, B. (1993). Can standard measures identify subclinical markers of autism? Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 23, 429–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. (1997). Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 27, 59–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D., McMahon, W., & Fillouz, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism: An informational processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 35, 1015–1031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. (1997). Dimensions of executive functions in normal, abnormal development. In N. Krasnegor, G. Lyon & P. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.), Development of the prefrontal cortex (pp. 265–281). Baltimore: Paul Brooks.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlstein, W., Carter, C., Barch, D., & Baird, J. (1998). The stroop task and attention deficits in Schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of card and single-trial Stroop methodologies. Neuropsychology, 12, 414–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Psychological Corporation. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapoport, J., Giedd, J., Blumenthal, J., Hamburger, S., Jeffries, N., Fernandez, T., et al. (1999). Progressive cortical change during adolescence in childhood-onset schizophrenia: A longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 649–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raymaekers, R., van der Meere, J., & Roeyers, H. (2004). Event-rate manipulation and its effect on arousal modulation and response inhibition in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26, 74–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: A study with functional MRI. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 891–896.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmond, C., de Haan, M., Friston, K., Gadian, D., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2003). Investigating individual differences in brain abnormalities in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 405–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxena, S., Brody, A., Schwartz, J., & Baxter, L. (1998). Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 26–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatz, A., Weimer, A., & Trauner, D. (2002). Brief report: Attention differences in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 32, 333–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller, P. (1966). Developmental study of color-word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 105–108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sichel, J., & Chandler, K. (1969). The color-word interference test: The effects of varied color-word combinations upon verbal response latency. Journal of Psychology, 72, 219–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. (1992). Biological and psychological development of executive functions. Brain & Cognition, 20, 8–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M., Pennington, B., Ozonoff, S., Rouse, B., & McCabe, E. (1990). Neuropsychology of early-treated phenylketonuria: Specific executive function deficits. Child Development, 61, 1697–1713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zilbovicius, M., Garreau, B., Samson, Y., Remy, P., Barthelemy, C., Syrota, A., et al. (1995). Delayed maturation of the frontal cortex in childhood autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 248–252.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shawn E. Christ.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Christ, S.E., Holt, D.D., White, D.A. et al. Inhibitory Control in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 1155–1165 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0259-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0259-y

Keywords

Navigation