Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Teaching to the Test…or Testing to Teach: Exams Requiring Higher Order Thinking Skills Encourage Greater Conceptual Understanding

  • Research into Practice
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In order to test the effect of exam-question level on fostering student conceptual understanding, low-level and high-level quizzes and exams were administered in two sections of an introductory biology course. Each section was taught in a high-level inquiry based style but was assigned either low-level questions (memory oriented) on the quizzes and exams, or high-level questions (application, evaluation, and analysis) on the quizzes and exams for the entirety of the semester. A final exam consisting of 20 low-level and 21 high-level questions was given to both sections. We considered several theoretical perspectives based on testing effect, test expectancy, and transfer-appropriate processing literature as well as the theoretical underpinnings of Bloom’s taxonomy. Reasoning from these theoretical perspectives, we predicted that high-level exams would encourage not only deeper processing of the information by students in preparation for the exam but also better memory for the core information (learned in the service of preparing for high-level questions). Results confirmed this prediction, with students in the high-level exam condition demonstrating higher performance on both the low-level final-exam items and the high-level final exam items. This pattern suggests that students who are tested throughout the semester with high-level questions acquire deep conceptual understanding of the material and better memory for the course information, and lends support to the proposed hierarchical nature of Bloom’s taxonomy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Analysis without the covariate (ANOVA) indicates the same effects, F (1, 167) = 13.04, MSE = .01, p < .0001, hp 2 = .072

  2. Again, analysis without the covariate (ANOVA) indicates the same effects, F (1, 167) = 7.73, MSE = 002, p < .01, hp 2 = .044

  3. Again, analysis without the covariate (ANOVA) indicates the same effects, F (1, 167) = 12.27, MSE = .02, p < .01, hp 2 = .068

  4. We appreciate an anonymous reviewer for raising this possibility.

References

  • AAAS. (2010). Vision and change: a call to action. Washington: AAAS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, G. L., Krathwohl, D. R., Airasian, P. W., Cruikshank, K. A., Mayer, R. E., Pintrich, P. R., et al. (Eds.). (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, H. S., Geer, B., & Hughes, E. C. (1968). Making the grade: The academic side of college life. New Brunswick: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biggs, J. B. (1987). Student approaches to studying and learning. Hawthorn: Australian Council for Educational Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S. (1984). Taxonomy of educational objectives. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, R. (1993). An instructional model for science education: Developing biological literacy. Colorado Springs: Biological Sciences Curriculum Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, N. A., & Reece, J. B. (2005). Biology, 7th ed. San Francisco: Benjamin Cummings.

  • Carpenter, S. K. (2012). Testing enhances the transfer of learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 279–283. doi:10.1177/0963721412452728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2006). Impoverished cue support enhances subsequent retention: Support for the elaborative retrieval explanation of the testing effect. Memory & Cognition, 34(2), 268–276. doi:10.3758/Bf03193405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., & Pashler, H. (2007). Testing beyond words: Using tests to enhance visuospatial map learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 474–478. doi:10.3758/Bf03194092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., Pashier, H., Wixted, J. T., & Vul, E. (2008). The effects of tests on learning and forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 36(2), 438–448. doi:10.3758/Mc.36.2.438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, S. K., Pashler, H., & Cepeda, N. J. (2009). Using tests to enhance 8th grade students’ retention of US history facts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 760–771. doi:10.1002/Acp.1507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrier, M., & Pashler, H. (1992). The influence of retrieval on retention. Memory & Cognition, 20(6), 633–642. doi:10.3758/Bf03202713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, J. C. K., & McDermott, K. B. (2007). The testing effect in recognition memory: A dual process account. [Reports - Evaluative]. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(2), 431–437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowe, A., Dirks, C., & Wenderoth, M. P. (2008). Biology in bloom: Implementing Bloom’s Taxonomy to enhance student learning in biology. CBE - Life Sciences Education, 7, 368–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickie, L. O. (2003). Approach to learning, the cognitive demands of assessment, and achievement in physics. [Reports - Research]. Canadian Journal of Higher Education, 33(1), 87–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finley, J. R., & Benjamin, A. S. (2012). Adaptive and qualitative changes in encoding strategy with experience: Evidence from the test-expectancy paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 38(3), 632–652. doi:10.1037/A0026215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. P., & Craik, F. I. M. (1977). Interaction between encoding and retrieval operations in cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 3(6), 710–711.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlen, W., & Deakin Crick, R. (2002). A systematic review of the impact of summative assessment and tests on students’ motivation for learning (EPPI-Centre Review, version 1.1*). Research Evidence in Education Library (Vol. 1). London: EPPI-Centre, Social Science Research Unit, Institute of Education.

  • Hill, P. W., & Mcgaw, B. (1981). Testing the simplex assumption underlying Blooms Taxonomy. American Educational Research Journal, 18(1), 93–101. doi:10.3102/00028312018001093.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. I., & Mayer, R. E. (2009). A testing effect with multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 621–629. doi:10.1037/A0015183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joughin, G. (2010). The hidden curriculum revisited: A critical review of research into the influence of summative assessment on learning. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 35(3), 335–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2007). Test format and corrective feedback modulate the effect of testing on memory retention. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 528–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy: An overview. Theory Into Practice, 41(4), 212–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kropp, R. P., et al., & Florida State Univ. Tallahassee. Inst. of Human Learning. (1966). The construction and validation of tests of the cognitive processes as described in the “Taxonomy of Educational Objectives”. (pp. 444).

  • Lawson, A. E. (1978). The development and validation of a classroom test of formal reasoning. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 15(1), 11–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E. (2002). Science teaching and development of thinking. Belmont: Wadsworth/Thompson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E., Alkhoury, S., Benford, R. B. C., & Falconer, K. A. (2000a). What kinds of scientific concepts exist? Concept construction and intellectual development in college biology. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(9), 996–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E., Alkhoury, S., Benford, R., Clark, B. R., & Falconer, K. A. (2000b). What kinds of scientific concepts exist? Concept construction and intellectual development in college biology. [Reports - Research]. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(9), 996–1018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E., Clark, B., Cramer-Meldrum, E., Falconer, K. A., Sequist, J. M., & Kwon, Y.-J. (2000c). Development of scientific reasoning in college biology: Do two levels of general hypothesis-testing skills exist? [Reports - Research]. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(1), 81–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madaus, G. F. (1973). A causal model analysis of Bloom’s Taxonomy. American Educational Research Journal, 10(4), 253–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2003). Learning and instruction. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall. 10(4), 253–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., & Donnelly, C. M. (1996). Learning with analogy and elaborative interrogation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 508–519. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.88.3.508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Blischak, D. M., & Challis, B. (1994). The effects of test expectancy on processing and memory of prose. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 230–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Anderson, J. L., Derbish, M. H., & Morrisette, N. (2007). Testing the testing effect in the classroom. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19(4-5), 494–513. doi:10.1080/09541440701326154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Wildman, K. M., & Anderson, J. L. (2012). Using quizzes to enhance summative-assessment performance in a web-based class: An experimental study. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 18–26.

  • McDaniel, M. A., Friedman, A., & Bourne, L. E., Jr. (1978). Remembering the levels of information in words. Memory & Cognition, 6, 156-164.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R. C., Agarwal, P. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2013). Quizzing in middle school science: Successful transfer performance on classroom exams. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 360–372.

  • McDermott, K. B., Agarwal, P. K., D’Antonio, L., Roediger, H. L., III, & McDaniel, M. A. (in press). Both multiple-choice and short-answer quizzes enhance later exam performance in middle and high school classes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied.

  • Miller, C. M. L., & Parlett, M. (1974). Up to the mark: A study of the examination game. London: Society for Research into Higher Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Momsen, J. L., Long, T. M., Wyse, S. A., & Ebert-May, D. (2010). Just the facts? Introductory undergraduate biology courses focus on low-level cognitive skills. [Reports - Evaluative]. CBE - Life Sciences Education, 9(4), 435–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Momsen, J. L., Offerdahl, E., Kryjevskaia, M., Montplaisir, L., Anderson, E., & Grosz, N. (2013). Using assessments to investigate and compare the nature of learning in undergraduate science courses. CBE - Life Sciences Education, 12, 239–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (1996). National science education standards. Washington: National Academy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17, 249–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, Agarwal, P. K., McDaniel, M. A., & McDermott, K. B. (2011). Test-enhanced learning in the classroom: Long-term improvements from quizzing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 382–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., Taylor, K., & Sholar, B. (2010). Tests enhance the transfer of learning. [Reports - Research]. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(1), 233–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seddon, G. M. (1978). The Properties of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives for the Cognitive Domain. Review of Educational Research, r.

  • Snyder, G. (1951). The hidden curriculum. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Zhang, L. (2008). Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 486–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Struyven, K., Dochy, F., & Janssens, S. (2005). Students’ perceptions about evaluation and assessment in higher education: A review. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 30(4), 325–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Wiley, J., & Griffing, T. D. (2011). Test expectancy affects metacomprehension accuracy. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 264–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007). The negative cascade of incongruent generative study-test processing in memory and metacomprehension. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 668–678. doi:10.3758/Bf03193305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Etten, S., Pressley, M., McInerney, D. M., & Liem, A. D. (2008). College senior’s theory of their academic motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 812–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins, G., McTighe, J., & Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development Alexandria VA. (1998). Understanding by Design. (pp. 214): Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development, 1703 North Beauregard Street, Alexandria, VA 22311–1714 (stock number 198199, members:.

  • Zawicki, P., & Witas, H. W. (2007). HIV-1 protecting CCR5-D32 allele in medieval Poland. Infection, Genetics and Evolution, 8(2): 146-151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zoller, U. (1993). Are lecture and learning compatible? Maybe for LOCS: unlikely for HOCS (SYM). Journal of Chemical Education 70: 195–197.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jamie L. Jensen.

Appendix

Appendix

Sample Test Items

Low-Level Unit Exam Items

Term/Concept: Haploid/Meiosis

  1. i.

    In humans, the haploid number, n equals:

    1. a)

      2n.

    2. b)

      44.

    3. c)

      23.

    4. d)

      46.

Term/Concept: Nondisjunction/Meiosis

  1. ii.

    Trisomy is a consequence of _______________________, the unequal distribution of chromosomes during meiosis.

    1. a)

      recombination

    2. b)

      cytokinesis

    3. c)

      nondisjunction

    4. d)

      crossing over

Term/Concept: Types of Selection/Evolution

  1. iii.

    Divergent selection occurs when:

    1. a)

      low-ranking, “sneaker” males disrupt the mating between a dominant female and a male.

    2. b)

      individuals with one extreme value of a trait have greater fitness than individuals with the other extreme value of the trait.

    3. c)

      individuals with extreme values of a trait have greater fitness than individuals with intermediate values of the trait.

    4. d)

      there is selection against the extreme ends of a trait’s distribution.

Term/Concept: Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium/Microevolution

The pygmies are a group of humans who are thought to have lived continuously in the rainforest of central Africa since the ice age, in geographically separated areas called refuges. There are two distinct groups of pygmies, the eastern and the western, living in two different refuges. The eastern population has an HbS allele frequency of 8 %. The western population has an HbS allele frequency of 18 %.

  1. iv.

    Are the eastern and western pygmies in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium?

    1. a)

      Yes

    2. b)

      No

    3. c)

      Impossible to tell

Term/Concept: Predicted Allelic Frequencies/Microevolution

  1. v.

    Given the HbS allele frequency in the western population is 0.18 (q), what is the frequency of the normal allele (HbA) in the western population?

    1. a)

      0.2

    2. b)

      0.18

    3. c)

      0.82

    4. d)

      0.08

Term/Concept: Interpreting the Periodic Table/Atomic theory

figure a
  1. vi.

    Looking at the figure to the right, how many protons does chlorine have?

    1. a)

      17

    2. b)

      18

    3. c)

      35.5

    4. d)

      71

    figure b

Term/Concept: Electron Shells/Atomic Bonding

  1. vii.

    If the normal electron-shell diagram of Sodium (Na) is pictured to the right, what does Na+ look like?

    figure c

Low-level Final Exam Items

Term/Concept: Types of Bonds/Atomic bonding

  1. viii.

    ____________ bonds deal with the sharing of electrons, while __________bonds deal with the transferring of electrons completely to another atom.

    1. a)

      Ionic, Hydrogen

    2. b)

      Ionic, Covalent

    3. c)

      Covalent, Hydrogen

    4. d)

      Covalent, Ionic

    5. e)

      Hydrogen, Covalent

    6. f)

      Hydrogen, Ionic

Term/Concept: G1/Cell Cycle

  1. ix.

    Within a Eukaryotic cell cycle, the primary stage during which normal cellular functioning occurs is

    1. a)

      G1

    2. b)

      S

    3. c)

      G2

    4. d)

      Mitosis

    5. e)

      Meiosis

Term/Concept: Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium/Microevolution

  1. x.

    Which of the following violations to Hardy–Weinberg assumptions would be most likely to change the allele frequency of a small population of pigmy monkeys living on an island?

    1. a)

      Mutation

    2. b)

      Changing environment

    3. c)

      Genetic drift

    4. d)

      Gene flow

    5. e)

      Selection

    6. f)

      Non-random mating

    7. g)

      All of the above have an equal chance of moving the population out of Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium.

High-Level Unit Exam Items

Term/Concept: Haploid/Meiosis

You and your spouse are expecting your first child. Because you and your spouse have an extensive history of genetic diseases on both sides of the family, you are concerned about the health of your unborn baby and request that an amniocentesis be performed so that you can prepare yourself for whatever lies ahead. During this procedure, a large needle is inserted through the mother’s belly and into the amniotic sack in order to collect a sample of amniotic fluid containing sloughed cells from the embryo. These cells are then grown in a laboratory for about a week in order to analyze the chromosomes.

  1. xi.

    The DNA from a typical human sperm cell weighs approximately 3.3 picograms (a picogram is a 10−12 g). If all chromosomes weighed approximately the same, how much does a typical chromosome weigh?

    1. a)

      0.07 picograms

    2. b)

      0.14 picograms

    3. c)

      0.28 picograms

    4. d)

      75.9 picograms

    5. e)

      151.8 picograms

Term/Concept: Nondisjunction/Meiosis

  1. xii.

    Doctors find that in the majority of embryo’s cells, the DNA weighs nearly 6.8 picograms. What is a possible explanation for this?

    1. a)

      The embryonic cells remained haploid, meaning that the sperm’s chromosomes never fused with the egg’s and were most likely kicked out with the polar body.

    2. b)

      The embryonic cells contain all duplicated chromosomes; thus, there must be a problem with Meiosis II and sister chromatids are never separating.

    3. c)

      The embryonic cells contain extra DNA indicating a possible trisomy.

    4. d)

      The embryo is normal but is definitely a girl, since girls carry an extra X chromosome.

Term/Concept: Types of Selection/Evolution

Zawicki and Witas (2007) studied the C-C chemokine receptor 5 (CCR5), a receptor on the white blood cell that is used by many pathogens to gain access to the cell, including HIV, Bubonic plague, and others. It has been found that a mutation (the deletion of 32 base pairs; designated CCR5-D32) prevents HIV from entering the cell. Individuals who are homozygous for the CCR5-D32 mutation are nearly completely resistant to HIV infection and individuals who are heterozygous have slower disease progression and better survival rates.

  1. xiii.

    There are no known disadvantages to being homozygous for the CCR5-D32 mutation. Predict what type of selection will most likely occur, given enough time?

    1. a)

      Stabilizing

    2. b)

      Directional

    3. c)

      Diversifying

Term/Concept: Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium/Microevolution

  1. xiv.

    Scientists have found a high incidence of this allele in European and Mediterranean people. The frequency of the CCR5-D32 allele on the European continent is 10 %. What percentage of the people are likely carriers of the CCR5-D32 allele?

    1. a)

      90 %

    2. b)

      10 %

    3. c)

      18 %

    4. d)

      9 %

Term/Concept: Predicted Allelic Frequencies/Microevolution

  1. xv.

    Given that the CCR5-Δ32 allele confers immunity to the bubonic plague, which of the following phenomena would you predict?

    1. a)

      The bubonic plague of the 14 th century would closely correspond in geographic distribution as well as timing with the suspected origin and increasing frequency of the CCR5- Δ32 mutation.

    2. b)

      The bubonic plague of the 14th century would directly precede (come before) the emergence of the CCR5- Δ32 mutation in the same geographic distribution indicating that the plague caused the mutation of the CCR5 receptor.

    3. c)

      The bubonic plague of the 14th century would be only in geographic areas where the CCR5- Δ32 mutation was not prevalent since those are the people most susceptible to plague infection.

    4. d)

      The bubonic plague of the 14th century would have emerged shortly after and in the same geographic location as the emergence and increasing frequency of the CCR5- Δ32 mutation indicating that the CCR5- Δ32 mutation increases plague infection rate.

Term/Concept: Electron Shells/Atomic Bonding

A salt is defined as “a compound resulting from the formation of an ionic bond; also called an ionic compound” (Campbell & Reece, 2005). That means that a salt can be formed from any two ions. Below are the electron-shell diagrams of magnesium (Mg) and chlorine (Cl).

figure d
  1. xvi.

    If table salt (the kind you eat) is sodium chloride (NaCl), what is the molecular formula of magnesium chloride?

    1. a)

      MgCl

    2. b)

      Mg2Cl

    3. c)

      MgCl 2

    4. d)

      Mg2Cl2

High-Level Final Exam Items

Term/Concept: Predicting Selection/Microevolution

  1. xvii.

    The incidence of spinal muscular atrophy (an autosomal recessive disease) in the United States is about 1 case in every 17,000. Whereas, in North Dakota, the prevalence is 1 in 6720. Which of the following would support the hypothesis that genetic drift was responsible for the increased allele frequency in North Dakota?

    1. a)

      There is an abnormally high concentration of mutagenic chemicals in the ground water causing an increased mutation rate in North Dakota.

    2. b)

      One of the best treatment centers for SMA is located in North Dakota causing migration of SMA carriers into the area at an abnormally high frequency.

    3. c)

      The original settlers of North Dakota were a small group of pioneers who happened to have an abnormally high frequency of the SMA allele in their population.

    4. d)

      A particular mosquito-born parasite native to North Dakota causes high infant mortality; carriers of the SMA allele are less likely to catch the disease.

Term/Concept: Electron Shells/Atomic Bonding

The hydrogen car runs off the power of hydrogen created from water in the process of electrolysis (i.e., the splitting of water). In this process, water is split into hydrogen and oxygen gases by running an electrical current through the water. This is done by placing two metal rods in the water and attaching them to an electrical source. The cathode donates electrons to the solution; whereas the anode accepts electrons from the solution. The reactions at each node are shown below (keep in mind that these reactions are happening at the same time):

At the Cathode, where hydrogen gas bubbles are produced: 2H+ + 2e- → H2 (gas)

At the Anode, where oxygen gas bubbles are produced: 2H2O → O2 (gas) + 4H+ + 4e-

  1. xviii.

    How many electrons must come from an outside source (like the anode or cathode) to make this reaction run?

    1. a)

      Two electrons are being donated by the cathode

    2. b)

      Two electrons are being donated by the anode

    3. c)

      Four electrons are being donated by the anode

    4. d)

      Eight electrons are being donated by the anode

    5. e)

      No electrons are being donated, they are all contained within the original water molecule

Term/Concept: Homologous Chromosomes/Meiosis

  1. xix.

    Cystic fibrosis, an autosomal recessive disease, is located on chromosome #7. If you viewed the 7th chromosome pair of an individual who is a carrier for cystic fibrosis during prophase of mitosis, which of the following is the best representation of what you would see?

    figure e

Term/Concept: Interpreting the Periodic Table/Atomic Theory

  1. xx.

    Let’s say I have a hypothetical element, called “Element J.” It tends to make three bonds and the most common isotope has 3 more neutrons than protons. Which of the following is the most accurate representation of Element J?

    figure f

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jensen, J.L., McDaniel, M.A., Woodard, S.M. et al. Teaching to the Test…or Testing to Teach: Exams Requiring Higher Order Thinking Skills Encourage Greater Conceptual Understanding. Educ Psychol Rev 26, 307–329 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9248-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9248-9

Keywords

Navigation