Skip to main content
Log in

Simultaneous GPS tracking reveals male associations in a solitary carnivore

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In hypercarnivorous species, females have large spatial requirements to meet their nutritional needs, and food competition among females is intense. As a result, females are typically solitary and territorial, and solitary males compete for access to dispersed females. Yet, largely anecdotal reports indicate that facultative male sociality may be more common in solitary carnivores than previously thought. We studied spatial interactions among fossas (Cryptoprocta ferox), Madagascar's largest carnivore, using simultaneous GPS tracking of 13 adult individuals to determine patterns of sex-specific spatial distribution and sociality. Male home ranges were larger than those of females, male home ranges overlapped more with those of other males than those of females with other females. Whereas some males were solitary, a subset of adult males was found to have very high home range overlap, high rates of co-location within <50 m, low minimum inter-individual distances, and significantly positive “dynamic interaction”. These associated dyads sometimes, but not always, were close relatives. The fact that solitary and associated males coexist in this population raises interesting questions concerning constraints and flexibility of social tolerance. This study yielded preliminary indications that female distribution appears to be primarily structured by resource competition, whereas male sociality seems to depend on demographic chance events, yet unknown proximate determinants of social tolerance, and it is associated with somatic and reproductive advantages. Male associations among carnivores are therefore more widespread and appear to be based on a wider range of factors than previously thought.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altmann J (1990) Primate males go where the females are. Anim Behav 39:193–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker PJ, Funk SM, Harris S, White PCL (2000) Flexible spatial organization of urban foxes, Vulpes vulpes, before and during an outbreak of sarcoptic mange. Anim Behav 59:127–146

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandeira de Melo LF, Lima Sábato MA, Vaz Magni EM, Young RJ, Coelho CM (2007) Secret lives of maned wolves (Chrysocyon brachyurus Illiger 1815): as revealed by GPS tracking collars. J Zool Lond 271:27–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barlow ACD, Smith JLD, Ahmad IU, Hossain ANM, Rahman M, Howlader A (2011) Female tiger Panthera tigris home range size in the Bangladesh Sundarbans: the value of this mangrove ecosystem for the species' conservation. Oryx 45:125–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff M, Daniels TJ, Gittleman JL (1984) Life history patterns and the comparative social ecology of carnivores. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 15:191–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertram BCR (1975) The social system of lions. Sci Am 232:54–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Börger L, Franconi N, De Michele G, Gantz A, Meschi F, Manica A et al (2006) Effects of sampling regime on the mean and variance of home range size estimates. J Anim Ecol 75:1393–1405

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calenge C (2006) The package “adehabitat” for the R software: a tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecol Model 197:516–519

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM (1994) Cheetahs of the Serengeti plains: group living in an asocial species. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM, Collins DA (1986) Male cheetahs of the Serengeti. Natl Geogr Res 2:75–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM, Stoner CJ (2003) The potential for interspecific competition among African carnivores. Biol Conserv 110:67–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr GM, Macdonald DW (1986) The sociality of solitary foragers: a model based on resource dispersion. Anim Behav 34:1540–1549

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavallini P (1996) Variation in the social system of the red fox. Ethol Ecol Evol 8:323–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavallini P, Nel JAJ (1990) Ranging behaviour of the Cape grey mongoose Galerella pulverulenta in a coastal area. J Zool 222:353–362

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock TH, Janson CH (2012) Primate socioecology at the crossroads: past, present, and future. Evol Anthropol 21:136–150

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA (1992) Potential reproductive rates and the operation of sexual selection. Q Rev Biol 67:437–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD (1982) Red deer: behavior and ecology of two sexes. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Creel S, Creel NM (1995) Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. Anim Behav 50:1325–1339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Creel S, Creel NM (1996) Limitation of African wild dogs by competition with larger carnivores. Conserv Biol 10:526–538

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crook JH (1970) Social organization and the environment: aspects of contemporary social ethology. Anim Behav 18:197–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crook JH, Gartlan JS (1966) Evolution of primate societies. Nature 210:1200–1203

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • da Silva J, Woodroffe R, Macdonald DW (1993) Habitat, food availability and group territoriality in the European badger, Meles meles. Oecologia 95:558–564

    Google Scholar 

  • da Silva J, Macdonald DW, Evans PGH (1994) Net costs of group living in a solitary forager, the Eurasian badger (Meles meles). Behav Ecol 5:151–158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalerum F (2005) Sociality in a solitary carnivore, the wolverine. Dissertation, Stockholm University

  • Dalerum F, Creel S, Hall SB (2006) Behavioral and endocrine correlates of reproductive failure in social aggregations of captive wolverines (Gulo gulo). J Zool 269:527–536

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dammhahn M, Kappeler PM (2009) Females go where the food is: does the socio-ecological model explain variation in social organisation of solitary foragers? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:939–952

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies NB, Hartley IR, Hatchwell BJ, Desrochers A, Skeer J, Nebel D (1995) The polygynandrous mating system of the alpine accentor, Prunella collaris. I. Ecological causes and reproductive conflicts. Anim Behav 49:769–788

    Google Scholar 

  • De Solla SR, Bonduriansky R, Brooks RJ (1999) Eliminating autocorrelation reduces biological relevance of home range estimates. J Anim Ecol 68:221–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Development Core Team R (2011) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna

    Google Scholar 

  • Doncaster CP (1990) Non-parametric estimates of interaction from radio-tracking data. J Theor Biol 143:431–443

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durant SM (1998) Competition refuges and coexistence: an example from Serengeti carnivores. J Anim Ecol 67:370–386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emlen ST, Oring LW (1977) Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197:215–223

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Feist JD, McCullough DR (1975) Reproduction in feral horses. J Reprod Fertil 23 (Suppl.):13–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Gannon WL, Sikes RS (2007) Guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild mammals in research. J Mammal 88:809–823

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geffen E, Hefner R, Macdonald DW, Ucko M (1992) Habitat selection and home range in the Blanford's fox, Vulpes cana: compatibility with the resource dispersion hypothesis. Oecologia 91:75–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehrt SD, Fritzell EK (1998) Resource distribution, female home range dispersion and male spatial interactions: group structure in a solitary carnivore. Anim Behav 55:1211–1227

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Getz WM, Wilmers CC (2004) A local nearest-neighbor convex-hull construction of home ranges and utilization distributions. Ecography 27:489–505

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gittleman JL (1984) The behavioural ecology of carnivores. PhD thesis, University of Sussex

  • Gittleman JL, Harvey PH (1982) Carnivore home-range size, metabolic needs and ecology. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10:57–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gompper ME, Gittleman JL (1991) Home range scaling: intraspecific and comparative trends. Oecologia 87:343–348

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins CE (1998) The behaviour and ecology of the fossa, Cryptoprocta ferox, (Carnivora: Viverridae) in a dry deciduous forest in western Madagascar. PhD thesis, University of Aberdeen.

  • Hawkins CE, Racey PA (2005) Low population density of a tropical forest carnivore, Cryptoprocta ferox: implications for protected area management. Oryx 39:35–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins CE, Racey PA (2008) Food habits of an endangered carnivore, Cryptoprocta ferox, in the dry deciduous forests of western Madagascar. J Mammal 89:64–74

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins CE, Racey PA (2009) A novel mating system in a solitary carnivore: the fossa. J Zool 277:196–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins CE, Dallas JF, Fowler PA, Woodroffe R, Racey PA (2002) Transient masculinization in the fossa, Cryptoprocta ferox (Carnivora, Viverridae). Biol Reprod 66:610–615

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hays WST, Conant S (2003) Male social activity in the small Indian mongoose Herpestes javanicus. Acta Theriol 48:485–494

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrera EA, Macdonald DW (1989) Resource utilization and territoriality in group-living capybaras (Hydrochoerus hydrochaeris). J Anim Ecol 58:667–679

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hooge PN, Eichenlaub B (2000) Animal movement extension to Arcview. Ver. 2.0. Alaska Science Center - Biological Science Office, U.S. Geological Survey, Anchorage, AK, USA

  • Jarman PJ (1974) The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology. Behaviour 48:215–267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenness J (2004) Weighted mean of points (weightmean.avx). Extension for ArcView 3.x, v. 1.2c. Jenness Enterprises

  • Johnson DDP, Macdonald DW, Newman C, Morecroft MD (2001) Group size versus territory size in group-living badgers: a large-sample field test of the resource dispersion hypothesis. Oikos 95:265–274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson DDP, Kays R, Blackwell PG, Macdonald DW (2002) Does the resource dispersion hypothesis explain group living? Trends Ecol Evol 17:563–570

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP (2002) Evolution of primate social systems. Int J Primatol 23:707–740

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiman DG, Eisenberg JF (1973) Comparisons of canid and felid social systems from an evolutionary perspective. Anim Behav 21:637–659

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klingel H (1975) Social organization and reproduction in equids. J Reprod Fertil 23(Suppl.):7–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruuk H (1972) The spotted hyena: a study of predation and social behavior. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindström E (1986) Territory inheritance and the evolution of group-living in carnivores. Anim Behav 34:1825–1835

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lührs ML, Dammhahn M (2010) An unusual case of cooperative hunting in a solitary carnivore. J Ethol 28:379–383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lührs ML, Dammhahn M, Kappeler PM (2013) Strength in numbers: males in a carnivore grow bigger when they associate and hunt cooperatively. Behav Ecol 24:21–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macdonald DW (1983) The ecology of carnivore social behaviour. Nature 301:379–384

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNab BK (1963) Bioenergetics and the determination of home range size. Am Nat 97:133–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mohr CO (1947) Table of equivalent populations of North American small mammals. Am Midl Nat 37:223–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundry R, Nunn CL (2009) Stepwise model fitting and statistical inference: turning noise into signal pollution. Am Nat 173:119–123

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pusey AE, Packer C (1987) Dispersal and philopatry. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 250–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasoloarison RM, Rasolonandrasana BPN, Ganzhorn JU, Goodman SM (1995) Predation on vertebrates in the Kirindy Forest, western Madagascar. Ecotropica 1:59–65

    Google Scholar 

  • Rathbun GB, Cowley TE (2008) Behavioural ecology of the black mongoose (Galerella nigrata) in Namibia. Mammal Biol 73:444–450

    Google Scholar 

  • Rood JP, Waser PW (1978) The slender mongoose (Herpestes sanguineus) in the Serengeti. Carnivore 1:54–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandell M (1989) The mating tactics and spacing patterns of solitary carnivores. In: Gittleman JL (ed) Carnivore behavior, ecology and evolution. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, pp 164–182

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schaller GB (1972) The Serengeti lion: a study of predator–prey relations. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Schülke O (2003) To breed or not to breed—food competition and other factors involved in female breeding decisions in the pair-living nocturnal fork-marked lemur (Phaner furcifer). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:11–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro SS, Wilk MB (1965) An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika 52:591–611

    Google Scholar 

  • Soisalo MK, Cavalcanti SMC (2006) Estimating the density of a jaguar population in the Brazilian Pantanal using camera-traps and capture-recapture sampling in combination with GPS radio-telemetry. Biol Conserv 129:487–496

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorg JP, Rohner U (1996) Climate and tree phenology of the dry deciduous forest of the Kirindy Forest. In: Ganzhorn JU, Sorg JP (eds) Ecology and economy of a tropical dry forest in Madagascar. Primate Report 46:57–80

  • Swihart RK, Slade NA (1985) Testing for independence of observations in animal movements. Ecology 66:1176–1184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terborgh J, Janson CH (1986) The socioecology of primate groups. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 17:111–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM (1996) The social systems of gregarious lemurs: lack of convergence with anthropoids due to evolutionary disequilibrium? Ethology 102:915–941

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner AP, Frank LG, Creel S (2008) Spatial grouping in behaviourally solitary striped hyaenas, Hyaena hyaena. Anim Behav 75:1131–1142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waser PM, Keane B, Creel SR, Elliott LF, Minchella DJ (1994) Possible male coalitions in a solitary mongoose. Anim Behav 47:289–294

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodroffe R, Macdonald DW (1993) Badger sociality—models of spatial grouping. Symp Zool Soc Lond 65:145–169

    Google Scholar 

  • Worton BJ (1989) Kernel methods for estimating the utilization distribution in home-range studies. Ecology 70:164–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG KA 1082/17-1), the Fossa Fund of Zoo Duisburg AG and the German Primate Center GmbH (DPZ). We thank Rémy Ampataka, Tianasoa Andrianjanahary, Nielsen Rabarijaona, and Jean-Pierre Tolojanahary for field assistance; Léonard Razafimanantsoa, Rodin Rasoloarison, and Heike Klensang for administrative support; Elise Huchard and Sandra Langer for veterinary assistance; Franz Kümmeth from e-obs GmbH for technical support; Melanie Dammhahn, Cornelia Kraus, Peter Waser, and three anonymous referees for very helpful comments. We thank the Département de Biologie Animale de l'Université d'Antananarivo, the Commission Tripartite CAFF, and CNFEREF Morondava for their authorization and support of this study.

Ethical standards

This study is in compliance with animal care regulations and applicable national laws of Germany and Madagascar. All research protocols were approved by the responsible authorities in Germany (Bundesministerium für Naturschutz, BfN) and Madagascar (Ministère de l'Environnement et des Eaux et Forêts, MINEEF).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter M. Kappeler.

Additional information

Communicated by K. E. Ruckstuhl

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Fig. 1

Selected plots of MCP home ranges per sex and month. Home ranges (colored lines) are presented within the Kirindy Forest (green contours) and marked individually (females F1–F4 on the left, males M1–M9 on the right). Ranges of the month stated are in thicker lines than those from other years (males) or months (females), which were included as additional information. For female ranges, resource information such as the course of a river (blue) and a central unforested area (green contour) was additionally provided. Female F4 (lower left) was tracked singly and therefore plotted with potential ranges of other females and its 50–90 % kernel ranges. For male ranges, additional information is provided for known and assumed locations of mating trees (red hash) in the area. Members of a male association are denoted with a “+” between their IDs. (JPEG 74 kb)

High-resolution image (TIFF 732 kb)

Fig. 2

Cumulative probabilities of observed (red circles) and expected (black line) inter-individual distances for simultaneously tracked female–female dyads. Expected values were calculated according to Doncaster (1990) from all possible n 2 distances between n positions of both individuals. (JPEG 24 kb)

High-resolution image (TIFF 206 kb)

Fig. 3

Cumulative probabilities of observed (red circles) and expected (black line) inter-individual distances for simultaneously tracked male–male dyads sorted by ID. Expected values were calculated according to Doncaster (1990) from all possible n 2 distances between n positions of both individuals. (JPEG 115 kb)

High-resolution image (TIFF 829 kb)

Fig. 4

Cumulative probabilities of observed (red circles) and expected (black line) inter-individual distances for simultaneously tracked female–male dyads sorted by ID of the female. Expected values were calculated according to Doncaster (1990) from all possible n 2 distances between n positions of both individuals. (JPEG 120 kb)

High-resolution image (TIFF 1,074 kb)

ESM Tables

(PDF 49 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lührs, M.L., Kappeler, P.M. Simultaneous GPS tracking reveals male associations in a solitary carnivore. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67, 1731–1743 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1581-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1581-y

Keywords

Navigation