Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 153, Issue 4, pp 614–627

Single-unit responses in the auditory cortex of monkeys performing a conditional acousticomotor task

Authors

  • Caroline Durif
    • Division of Physiology and Program in Neurosciences, Department of Medicine, Faculty of SciencesUniversity of Fribourg
  • Christophe Jouffrais
    • Division of Physiology and Program in Neurosciences, Department of Medicine, Faculty of SciencesUniversity of Fribourg
    • Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, CNRS-UPS UMR 5549Université Paul Sabatier
    • Division of Physiology and Program in Neurosciences, Department of Medicine, Faculty of SciencesUniversity of Fribourg
Research Article

DOI: 10.1007/s00221-003-1613-3

Cite this article as:
Durif, C., Jouffrais, C. & Rouiller, E.M. Exp Brain Res (2003) 153: 614. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1613-3

Abstract

The general goal of the present study was to assess the response properties to tones of single neurons in the auditory cortex (primary auditory area, A1, and middle lateral auditory belt, ML) of two macaque monkeys while performing an acousticomotor discrimination task requiring a controlled level of attention and motivation. For each neuron, an approximation of the frequency receptive field (FRF) was first established. Second, based on the FRF, sets of paired tone frequencies were defined in which two different tone frequencies had to be associated by the monkey, following a trial and error strategy, to a left or a right key-press with the left arm. After acquisition of the association, the two tones of the pair were presented randomly (“instruction stimulus”) and, if the monkey touched the correct key, the stimulus was repeated (“confirmation stimulus”) and a reward was delivered. The majority of units (63%) had a FRF formed by multiple peaks, whereas 25% and 12% of units exhibited a simple U-shaped FRF and a “mosaic” FRF, composed of several separated zones of response, respectively. Five principal response patterns were observed: On, Off, On-Off, Sustained, and Inhibition. In relation to the acousticomotor association task, some auditory cortical neurons (33%) exhibited a different response to the same stimulus when presented, in the same trials, as instruction or as confirmation. It was also observed that the response to the same instruction stimulus could differ when comparing correct trials with erroneous trials (wrong motor response). In conclusion, the response properties of auditory cortical neurons in behaving monkeys are strongly dependent on the physical parameters of sounds (frequency, intensity, etc.) as indicated by FRF characteristics, but a substantial influence of the behavioral context and performance may also play an important role.

Keywords

SensorimotorPrimateBehaviorTonesResponse patternReceptive field.

Introduction

Responsiveness to complex sounds in the auditory cortex was studied in nonhuman primates (anesthetized preparations: Rauschecker et al. 1995; Wang et al. 1995; Rauschecker 1998a, 1998b; Wang and Kadia 2001; Nagarajan et al. 2002; awake preparations: Manley and Muller-Preuss 1978; Glass and Wollberg 1979; Schwarz and Tomlinson 1990; Pelleg-Toiba and Wollberg 1991; Bieser and Muller-Preuss 1996; Bieser 1998; Fishman et al. 2000; Liang et al. 2002). To investigate more basic properties, responses to simple sound stimuli were examined, with emphasis on tonotopy in Old World (anesthetized preparations: Merzenich and Brugge 1973; Morel et al. 1993; Kosaki et al. 1997; Rauschecker et al. 1997; awake preparations: Recanzone et al. 2000a) and New World monkeys (anesthetized preparations: Imig et al. 1977; Aitkin et al. 1986; Luethke at al. 1989; Morel and Kaas 1992; Recanzone et al. 1999; awake preparations: Cheung et al. 2001a). A tonotopic arrangement has been observed in the primary auditory cortex, as well as in other auditory cortical areas in both cats and monkeys (Brugge and Merzenich 1973; Merzenich et al. 1975; Reale and Imig 1980; Aitkin et al. 1986; Cheung et al. 2001a). Experiments conducted in behaving macaque monkeys showed a considerable variability of other response properties across neurons, such as threshold, peak activity, latency, frequency tuning, and intensity function. These investigations did not reveal a clear functional specialization, but instead supported the notion that the auditory cortical areas differ in a probabilistic manner, in the sense that the proportions of the different response types varied from one area to the next (Recanzone et al. 2000a). Nevertheless, in the lateral belt areas, neurons’ preferences for complex sounds revealed a specificity of the anterolateral area for monkey calls, whereas the caudolateral area exhibited the greatest spatial selectivity (Rauschecker et al. 1995; Rauschecker and Tian 2000; Tian et al. 2001). These data, together with the parallel arrangement of the connections between the auditory cortex and the prefrontal cortex (Romanski et al. 1999a, 1999b; Kaas and Hackett 1999), were taken as evidence for a processing of the “what” and “where” attributes of acoustic stimuli along largely separate streams. Such segregation of information processing related to sound recognition, and sound localization is consistent with recent data derived from human subjects (Maeder et al. 2001; Adriani et al. 2003).

Tonotopy is an important organizational principle (Calford et al. 1983), but it is only one of many stimulus dimensions potentially encoded in the auditory cortex. For instance, a neuron is also defined by its frequency selectivity, determined by its “frequency tuning curve” (FTC). Fairly stereotyped FTCs were recorded from primary auditory neurons but, at progressively higher stages of the auditory pathways, neurons exhibit more and more complex FTCs (Recanzone et al. 2000b; Sutter 2000). Frequency selectivity was demonstrated to be substantially modified by the level of anesthesia (Pfingst et al. 1977; Benson et al. 1981; Zurita et al. 1994; Kisley and Gerstein 1999; Cheung et al. 2001b; Gaese and Ostwald 2001) and, in awake animals, by different conditions of attention or of learning (Pfingst et al. 1977; Benson et al. 1981; Diamond and Weinberger 1989; Bakin and Weinberger 1990; Edeline and Weinberger 1993; Edeline et al. 1993; Weinberger et al. 1993; Ohl and Scheich 1997; Ahissar et al. 1998; Kisley and Gerstein 2001). Complex FTCs were particularly prominent in the auditory cortex of awake squirrel monkeys (Funkenstein and Winter 1973; Pelleg-Toiba and Wollberg 1989).

Criteria established in the cochlear nucleus (Kiang 1965; Kiang et al. 1965; Pfeiffer and Kiang 1965; Pfeiffer 1966; Rhode and Kettner 1987; Young et al. 1988) were applicable to most response patterns observed in the inferior colliculus of monkeys (Ryan and Miller 1977, 1978). Recently, Recanzone (2000a) examined whether the response pattern categorization elaborated in the CN can be transposed to single neurons in the primary auditory cortex (A1) of behaving macaque monkeys. Although some basic response patterns described in the CN were also observed in A1, the responses were rather found to form a continuum across several patterns, in contrast to discrete and well-separated classes. Furthermore, in sharp contrast to the CN, A1 neurons frequently exhibited either excitatory or inhibitory responses following the offset of the stimulus. In a study conducted in behaving macaque monkeys (Pfingst and O’Connor 1981), responses of auditory cortical neurons were described as phasic, when restricted to the onset of the stimulus, or tonic, when maintained throughout the stimulus duration.

The activity of neurons in the primate auditory cortex is strongly affected by the level of anesthesia, motivation, and the level of attention (Benson et al. 1981). In other words, it is likely that the response of an auditory cortical neuron may be modified if, instead of being stimulated passively, the animal expects the presentation of stimuli (Miller et al. 1972; Pfingst et al. 1977). Even more prominent effects may be expected if the stimulus carries crucial information for the selection of an appropriate behavioral response giving access to a reward (Hocherman et al. 1981). For this reason, a first aim of the present study was to establish the response patterns and tuning properties of single auditory cortical neurons of monkeys in the context of a complex acousticomotor delayed task requiring a sustained and controlled level attention and motivation of the animal.

Although the response properties of auditory cortical neurons are generally considered to be largely determined by the physical parameters of the stimulus (frequency and intensity for a pure tone), an influence of the behavioral dimension is likely. For instance, it has been shown that the response of a given auditory cortical neuron to a tone burst or a noise burst differs when the same stimulus instructs different motor responses (Vaadia et al. 1982). In order to extend these data, a comparable, but more elaborated acousticomotor task was designed in the present study, in which monkeys had to associate, by trial and error, tones with reaching movements aimed to different targets while recording neuronal activity in the auditory cortex. This paradigm allowed us to investigate different facets of the influence of the behavioral context on the responses of auditory cortex neurons to behaviorally significant acoustic stimuli. More specifically, the two following questions will be addressed. Does a tone generate a similar neuronal response when presented as “instructing” stimulus or as stimulus “confirming” that the motor selection was correct (context effect)? Does the neuronal response to a tone instructing a motor selection differ whether the animal produced a correct or an erroneous behavioral response (“predictive” effect)?

Methods

Animals

Experiments were conducted on two rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), 3.5 years old (MK1) and 9 years old (MK2), weighing 4 and 8 kg, respectively. Sucrose and grain-based pellets were used as the primary reward during the training and experimental sessions. At no time were the monkeys deprived of food, but they proved to be sufficiently motivated to execute daily a number of trials ranging from 300 to 500. Rewards (pellets of grain-based and purified diets) obtained during the daily sessions were the first food access of the day. Additional food (animals’ maintenance food, fruit and cereals) was provided after the daily sessions. The animals had free access to water, and their body weight was checked daily and remained stable. The experimental protocols were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (1996), the European Community’s Guidelines for Animal Protection and Use for Experimentation, and approved by the Swiss veterinary authorities.

Stimulus generation and delivery

Experiments were conducted in a double-walled, sound-attenuation room (Eckel audiometric booth, type AB200), and stimuli were delivered under free-field conditions through two MLi-691P speakers located 0.4 m lateral to the monkey’s head. The sound-delivery system was calibrated with a sound-level meter (Brüel and Kjaer, 2231), using a microphone (Brüel and Kjaer, 4155; prepolarized free-field half-inch) placed at the position of the animal’s left and right ears. Stimuli were generated digitally using the Virtual Waves 2.23 software, shaped routinely into 500-ms bursts, with 10 ms rise and fall time. The standard stimuli consisted of pure-tone bursts (11 frequencies available: 0.2, 0.4, 0.8, 1.2, 1.6, 2.4, 3.2, 4.8, 6.4, 9.6, and 12.8 kHz). Additional stimuli included tone sweeps (500 ms; up and down) covering narrow frequency ranges (0.25–1 kHz, 1–5 kHz, 5–10 kHz, 10–15 kHz, 15–20 kHz), complex sounds (including species-specific vocalizations) and white-noise bursts.

Behavioral task

The monkeys were initially trained over 10-12 months to execute an acousticomotor conditional task with the head unrestrained. It should be noted here that the training of macaque monkeys to a conditional motor task instructed acoustically turned out to be much longer than training to a motor task based on visual instructions (Kermadi et al. 1998; Wannier et al. 2002). During each daily session, the animal was exposed to several sets of paired pure tones delivered at 60 dB SPL. In each set, two different tone frequencies were presented and, by trial and error, the monkey had to associate each of the two tones to a specific motor response, namely a left or a right key-touch performed with the left arm. After acquisition of the association (usually 90% of correct responses), obtained usually after 5-10 trials, the two tone bursts were presented randomly. After 12 correct trials for each paired stimulus, a new set of two frequencies had to be acquired by the monkey.

The monkey faced a vertical transparent glass, in front of a monitor screen. Below the monitor, at arm’s distance, a horizontal touch-sensitive pad was used by the animal to place the left hand after a white square appeared on the screen, triggering the initiation of a trial (Fig. 1A). In front of the monkey and next to the initiation pad, a pellet basin was used to distribute the reward. The transparent glass was adorned by a panel composed of two key-detectors corresponding to the targets for the monkey’s motor response. The initiation of the trial was followed by a random delay, ranging from 1,200 to 2,500 ms, before the presentation of one of the two tones of the set, corresponding to the instruction stimulus (IS). After a second random delay (ranging from 400 to 1,400 ms), a visual go-signal (gray rectangle) appeared on the top of the monitor, instructing the animal to execute the motor response (left or right key-touch). If the monkey touched the correct key, the stimulus was repeated, corresponding to the so-called confirmation stimulus (CS). Finally, a reward was delivered 200 ms after the CS. On the other hand, if the monkey touched the wrong key an error-signal was delivered (dark-blue rectangle displayed in the middle of the monitor) and the trial was aborted. In MK1, in some recording sessions, the visual error-signal was accompanied by presentation of the tone associated to the wrong key, thus corresponding to an acoustic error-signal. The monkey was given a maximum time of 6-7 s to perform the entire sequence, although the duration of the trials usually ranged from 4 to 5.5 s (Fig. 1B).
Fig. 1A, B.

Schematic representation of a typical (correct) trial (A) and its time structure (B). A Schematic representation of the experimental setup. The monkey, sat in its primate chair, faced a vertical transparent glass, in front of a monitor screen. Below the monitor, a horizontal touch-sensitive pad (bottom left, gray rectangle) was used as start-lever for initiating a trial. Next to the start-lever, a pellet basin was used to deliver the reward (not shown). The transparent glass is adorned by a panel composed of two key-detectors (black squares corresponding to the left and right motor response targets). B Schematic representation of the temporal sequence of events during a typical trial of the acousticomotor task (see text for details). Row 1 indicates the position of the different events imposed to the monkey (stimuli, variable delays, etc.). Row 2 illustrates the monkey’s motor behavior. From trial initiation, the monkey maintained the left hand on the start pad until the go-signal, followed by a reaction time (RT) preceding the pointing movement to the selected target. (IS instructing stimulus, CS confirmation stimulus)

Surgery

When the monkeys reached a daily stable level of performance (80–90% of correct responses), a head-fixation bar in titanium (63 mm long, 4–8 mm wide, and 10 mm high) was implanted longitudinally above the midline. The head fixation device was anchored to the skull using 10 titanium screws and orthopedic cement (Palacos R-40 + 500 mg gentamicin). The monkey was then retrained in order to perform the task having the head fixed. When the daily stable level of performance was recovered, a cylindrical, stainless steel recording chamber (skull-mounting chamber—Narishige MO-95 skull mounting hydraulic microdrive) was implanted vertically on the left side, to allow subsequent vertical electrode penetrations to reach the left auditory cortex. A CT scan of the monkey’s head was performed before implantation in order to optimize the positioning of the recording chamber, together with the stereotaxic landmarks. The shape of the head-fixation device was also adjusted based on the shape of the skull as seen on the CT scan.

Surgeries for the implantation of the head-fixation bar and recording chamber were performed as follows under aseptic conditions. Antibiotics (Albipen: ampicillin 10%, 15–30 mg/kg s.c. or i.m.) were given before the surgery and for 5 days postoperatively, as well as the analgesic carprofen (Rimadyl, 4 mg/kg s.c.) to prevent pain. Anesthesia was induced by injection of ketamine hydrochloride (Ketalar, 5 mg/kg i.m.). Atropine sulfate (0.05 ml/kg i.m.), dexamethasone (Decadron, 0.05 ml/kg diluted 1:1 in saline, i.m.) and the analgesic Carprofen were then administered. During implantation, performed under deep anesthesia maintained by perfusion of propofol 1% (Fresenius, 3 ml/kg per hour i.v.), heart and respiration rates, rectal temperature, O2 blood saturation, and expiratory CO2 content were continuously monitored. A craniotomy was performed in order to expose the dura above the relevant cortical region (centered 10 mm AP and 25 mm lateral, based on coordinates derived from CT scans of the two monkeys and the rhesus monkey brain atlas; Paxinos et al. 2000). The chamber (18 mm inside-diameter) was anchored to the skull with titanium-screws and cemented with orthopedic cement. A stainless steel cap was screwed onto the chamber to seal it in between recording sessions. The dura was left intact, the muscles and the skin were then sutured. The animals were allowed at least 2 weeks to recover before the recording sessions began. During the entire recording period, the head and the chamber were cleaned daily with a iodine solution (Betadine) and locally treated with an antibiotic (Nebacetin: neomycin and bacitracin).

Recording procedure

During the recording session, the monkey sat in a primate chair and was observed via a video camera. Action potentials were recorded extracellularly with tungsten microelectrodes (FHC, Brunswick, Maine, USA; impedance adjusted to 2–4 MΩ at 1 kHz). The microelectrode was advanced vertically to the auditory cortex using a skull-mounting hydraulic microdrive system (Narishige MO-95), fixed to the chronically implanted chamber (Pfingst and O’Connor 1980). A cannula guide, added to the drive system, ensured vertical penetrations. The microelectrode was advanced successively through the dura, the parietal cortex, the white matter, the dorsal bank of the lateral sulcus, crossed the lateral sulcus itself, where an absence of neural activity was observed, to finally access the ventral bank of the lateral sulcus. Recordings were made at depths ranging from approximately 7 to 19 mm below the pial surface. Activity was recorded from single neurons in the left auditory cortex. After recording a neuron, a minimum displacement of the electrode of 250 μm was made before searching another cell. Neuronal activity was amplified (×10,000; AC differential amplifiers, model MDA-4; BAK Electronics), filtered (0.5–5 kHz) and displayed on an oscilloscope. The action potentials were discriminated from background activity with an amplitude discriminator system (FHC, type Slope/Height; ×4), and the resulting trigger pulses were stored on-line in digital form (resolution 1 ms) on a PC computer using the freeware data acquisition software Cortex (developed at the NIMH: http://www.cortex.salk.edu). The same software allowed presentation of the visual and acoustic stimuli, as well as the control of the behavioral task.

Following isolation of a single unit, the 11 frequency tone bursts were delivered to the monkey at 5 intensities (40, 50, 60, 70, and 80 dB SPL), in order to determine the patterns of response to tones and the FRF. Each tone burst at a given frequency and intensity was repeated 3–5 times along a pseudorandom sequence. Two consecutive tone bursts were separated by a period of silence of 300 ms. Based on the FRF, 3–5 tones were selected in order to define sets of 2 frequencies that would be used for the behavioral task (see above). If, after completion of the behavioral task, the unit was still present, the procedure of FRF determination was repeated.

Data analysis

Data collected during the FRF assessment were analyzed off line, using raster displays and peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs), in which trials were aligned to the onset of the tone bursts. In contrast to the trials recorded during the behavioral task (see below), the rapid pseudorandom presentation of the tone bursts did not allow establishment quantitatively of an “activity of reference” of the unit. Therefore, the FRF and the patterns of response were determined on the basis of visual inspection of the PSTHs and dot rasters, by comparing the activity during and preceding the tone bursts. Response patterns were classified into five groups: (1) an “On” response was defined as a phasic increase in discharge rate within the first 200 ms of the tone burst; (2) an “Off” response corresponded to a phasic increase in activity following the offset of the stimulus; (3) a combination of the two responses above was referred to as an “On–Off” response; (4) a “sustained” response was defined as an increase in discharge rate maintained throughout the 500 ms of the stimulus presentation; (5) a decrease in activity during the tone burst was referred to as an “inhibition”.

The behavioral performance was assessed using trigger signals corresponding to discrete events of the task, generated by sensors (pad pressed or released, touching of target, delivery of reward, etc.). Neuronal activity was analyzed off-line, using raster displays and PSTHs, in which the trials were aligned to different events. A statistical criterion was applied in order to establish whether an activity was considered as a significant modulation in relation to the task. A mean baseline activity (“activity of reference”) of the neuron and its standard deviation (SD) were calculated, while the animal was sitting quietly with the left hand on the touch-sensitive pad, on a 600-ms window of activity taken in the random delay preceding the IS. A level of activity which was higher, or lower, than the mean activity of reference ±3 SDs was considered as a significant task-related increase (excitation) or decrease (inhibition), respectively, of firing rate.

Results

Extracellular activity was recorded from a total of 82 auditory cortical neurons responsive to tones, sampled along 56 electrode penetrations (n=40 in MK1 and n=16 in MK2) aimed mainly toward the core of the auditory cortex, principally to A1 (Rauschecker et al. 1997; Hackett et al. 2001). These units were all tested for their responses to tones, as determined by the presentation of the 11 frequencies available. Among them, a subgroup of 45 units were recorded sufficiently long in order to perform the behavioral paradigm.

For each monkey, a surface map of the stereotaxic coordinates of all electrode penetrations was established. Electrode penetrations were distant from each other by 0.5 mm along the rostrocaudal and mediolateral axes. In both monkeys, the zone on the surface map containing all electrode penetrations covered a rostrocaudal extent of 6 mm and 5 mm along the mediolateral axis. A reconstruction of electrode penetrations and determination of unit location on histological sections was not possible in MK1 due to a massive hemorrhage that occurred after a recording session, along the electrode track while crossing the parietal lobe. As a result, a considerable distortion of the left hemisphere took place, affecting substantially the auditory cortex.

After killing MK2, postmortem landmarks were made, based on the stereotaxic coordinate system, in order to transpose the position of the surface map on histological sections. In addition, marks left by electrode penetrations performed during the few sessions preceding the death of the animal were observed on histological sections. The most caudal electrode penetrations were located at the rostrocaudal stereotaxic coordinate 4.2 mm (as defined in the atlas of the rhesus monkey brain; Paxinos et al. 2000), corresponding to the caudal limit of the auditory koniocortex (areas AKL and AKM), considered also as the caudal limit of A1; Hackett et al. 1998). Considering a rostrocaudal extent of 10 mm for A1 (Hackett et al. 1998), all our electrode penetrations were thus comprised within the rostrocaudal extent of A1. Mediolaterally, transposed on the atlas of the monkey brain, the electrode penetrations were located between the stereotaxic coordinates 21 and 26 mm from midline (between 22 and 25 mm for the most rostral electrode tracks). This general location indicates that the majority of the electrode penetrations in MK2 are in the auditory koniocortex (in other words, in A1), although a few of them (the most lateral ones) may have rather crossed the lateral belt, more precisely the middle lateral auditory belt (ML), as defined by Hackett et al. (1998).

As explained above, histological reconstruction was not possible in MK1. However, the area covered by the electrode penetrations was similar (6 mm by 5 mm) and the same stereotaxic targets were aimed. In addition, the response characteristics were comparable between the two monkeys. Therefore, we tend to assume that the distribution of the electrode penetrations was comparable in MK1, in other words focused mainly to A1 and, to a lesser extent, ML. In the present report, reference to units located in the “auditory cortex” thus means belonging to the areas A1 or ML.

Assessment of FRF and response patterns

As explained in the Methods section, a pseudorandom sequence of 55 tone bursts (11 frequencies at 5 intensity levels) was delivered 3–5 times while the monkey was waiting for the initiation of the rewarded behavioral task. A typical response of an auditory cortical neuron to the eleven frequencies presented at 2 intensities (40 and 70 dB SPL) is shown in Fig. 2. This neuron exhibited a complex behavior, as reflected by a response present at several frequencies and intensities tested. In addition, the patterns of response were quite variable across frequency and intensity. Representative examples of the five main temporal patterns of response to tones (On, Off, On–Off, sustained and inhibition) are shown in Fig. 3.
Fig. 2A, B.

Responses of an auditory cortical cell to 11 tone frequencies (indicated in hertz for each graph), delivered at 40 (A) and 70 dB SPL (B), as seen on dot rasters and corresponding peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs; 30-ms bins). In the dot rasters, each row represents a single trial and each tick mark represents the occurrence of an action potential. The horizontal axis is time (seconds) and the vertical axis of the PSTHs corresponds to the number of spikes per second. All trials were aligned on the onset of the stimulus (time zero, indicated by the thin vertical line). The solid bar below each dot raster indicates the stimulus duration (500 ms)

Fig. 3.

Five neurons show, in the form of dot rasters and PSTHs, the patterns of response to tone bursts observed in the present study: On, Off, On–Off, sustained, and inhibition (see text). Same conventions as in Fig. 2. For each neuron, the stimulus was a pure tone at its best frequency (BF)

The variability of response patterns to tone bursts observed for a given neuron is illustrated in more detail in Figs. 4, 5, 6. First, response patterns were influenced by the intensity. In response to pure-tone bursts at best frequency (BF), increasing the intensity from 40 to 80 dB SPL led to a change from an On response at 40 and 50 dB SPL to an On–Off response at 60, 70, and 80 dB SPL (Fig. 4). Another neuron displayed a progressive change from an On to a sustained response when the intensity was increased from 40 to 70 dB SPL (Fig. 5). Second, it is well known that, at a fixed intensity, the response pattern may vary with frequency. The neuron in Fig. 6 exhibited a limited frequency domain of selectivity characterized by a strong, sustained excitation at 200 Hz, whereas at 400 Hz the response was limited to an onset component (On response). No clear response was observed at other frequencies. Overall, with respect to their patterns of response to tones, 3.8% of neurons were purely intensity dependent, 11.5% of neurons were purely frequency dependent, whereas the vast majority of them (53.8%) were both frequency and intensity dependent. In the latter case, this means that more than one pattern of response was observed at a given frequency and, similarly, more than one pattern of response was observed at a given intensity. The remaining tone responsive neurons (30.8%) showed a response pattern that was not clearly affected by either the frequency or the intensity of the tone bursts. The only change was, at most, a gradual increase or decrease in discharge rate without change of response pattern and/or a narrowing in the effective range of frequencies as the intensity of the stimulus was reduced. Due to this variability of response patterns observed for a single neuron, it was meaningless to classify neurons based on that property. Nevertheless, for a subpopulation of cells where a BF could be tentatively defined (n=68), the units were distributed according to their response patterns observed at BF and at threshold (or at 40 dB SPL). The following distribution was obtained: 48 (70.6%) On units, 6 (8.8%) Off units, 6 (8.8%) On–Off units, 6 (8.8%) sustained units, and 2 (3%) inhibition units.
Fig. 4.

Responses of a single auditory cortical cell to a tone burst at best frequency (BF; 400 Hz) presented at five intensities, illustrating the dependency of response pattern onto intensity. Same conventions as in Fig. 2

Fig. 5.

Responses of a single auditory cortical cell to a tone burst at BF (200 Hz) presented at four intensities, illustrating in the form of dot rasters a gradual change of response strength, from On to sustained. Collection of data began for each trial at the time point indicated by the arrowhead. The response at the lowest (40 dB SPL) and highest (70 dB SPL) intensities are also represented by a PSTH. Same conventions as in Fig. 2

Fig. 6.

Dot rasters illustrate the responses of a single auditory cortical cell to tone bursts of different frequency presented at a stable intensity of 80 dB SPL. The responses to 200-Hz and 400-Hz tone bursts are represented in the form of PSTHs also. Same conventions as in Figs. 2 and 5

FRFs were approximated in 77 units by plotting with different symbols the response pattern observed at a given combination of frequency and intensity among the 55 tone bursts tested. Different symbols were used to distinguish the five response patterns (Fig. 7). Finally, an outline surrounding all symbols was drawn, thus approximating the FRF (Fig. 7). Units were distributed in 3 groups based on the shape of their FRF: single FRF with multiple peaks (group I in Fig. 7A), single FRF with one peak or U shape FRF (group II in Fig. 7B) and mosaic FRF (group III in Fig. 7C). The latter group was characterized by the presence of separated zones of frequency sensitivity. The absolute numbers of groups I, II, and III FRFs are given in Table 1 and they represented 63.6%, 24.7% and 11.7% of all units, respectively.
Fig. 7.

Three single auditory cortical neurons illustrate the three types (groups) of FRFs, as defined in the text. The response patterns observed at each combination of frequency and intensity are distinguished by different symbols. Note the variability of response patterns observed within the FRF of the two units representative of the groups I and III

Table 1.

Number of response patterns in the frequency receptive field (FRF). For each FRF group, the number and percentage of units exhibiting 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5 different response patterns within their FRF are indicated. The sum of all percentages in each FRF group is 100%. Note that FRF group I units tend to have more variability of response patterns within their FRF than the other two groups

FRF groups

Response patterns

Units (n=77)

1

2

3

4

5

n

%

n

%

n

%

n

%

n

%

n

%

Group I

12

24.6

6

12.2

28

57.1

3

6.1

49

100

Group II

8

42.1

11

57.9

19

100

Group III

5

55.6

2

22.2

2

22.2

9

100

Group II units, characterized by a restricted zone of frequency sensitivity, exhibited in general a relatively limited variability of response pattern (Table 1). Indeed, group II units presented only one (42.1%) or two (57.9%) patterns of response across the FRF. In contrast, as illustrated in Fig. 7, most group I units (57.1%) exhibited three response patterns across the FRF. Group III units presented a variation across their FRF with either two (22.2%) or four (22.2%) patterns of response (Table 1). A typical, complex variation of response patterns is illustrated for a group I unit in Fig. 8. This unit was responsive to two frequency domains at low intensity (40 dB SP), namely from 200 to 1,200 Hz and from 4,800 to 9,600 Hz. These two frequency domains became larger at 50 dB SPL and finally merged at higher intensity (60 and 70 dB SPL). Note that at least three patterns of response can be seen: On, On–Off, and sustained.
Fig. 8.

Responses of a single auditory cortical cell to 44 combinations of frequency and intensity, illustrating a group I FRF and the variability of response patterns. Same conventions as in Figs. 2 and 5

Behavioral task

A total of 45 neurons could be recorded long enough in order to perform the behavioral paradigm for at least one set of 2 frequencies presented in the 2 epochs in which each frequency was associated successively to the left and right targets, as explained in the Methods section. Behavioral data regarding the reaction time (RT) and the movement time (MT) were derived from the sessions in which the activity of the 45 neurons included in the present analysis was recorded. As illustrated in Fig. 1, RT is the time separating the go-signal from the initiation of the movement of the left hand. MT is the time interval between the initiation of the movement of the left hand and the touch of the left or right target. RT and MT values are given in Table 2, separately for the two monkeys. It appeared that MK1 exhibited statistically significant shorter RTs and MTs than MK2 (P<0.01), with the exception of the RT for the left target trials (P=0.074). In general, SDs were relatively small (Table 2), indicating that the behavioral performance was stable, suggesting that the levels of attention and motivation were fairly constant. RT values were in a range consistent with previous experiments in our laboratory, where monkeys performed a visuomotor unimanual conditional task (Kermadi et al. 1998), in which RTs ranged from 220 to 286 ms. Therefore, RTs appear to be comparable for unimanual motor tasks instructed visually or acoustically. There was no difference between trials aimed toward the left target versus the right target, except for the RTs in MK1 (Table 2).
Table 2.

Behavioural data given by mean values of reaction times and movement times

Reaction time (ms)

Movement time (ms)

Left target

Right target

Left target

Right target

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Mean

SD

Monkey 1

251*

21

231

9

203

17

222

20

Monkey 2

282

39

275

40

316

9

310

24

*Significant difference of reaction times between trials aimed to the left target versus trials aimed to the right target: P<0.009 (SYSTAT: two-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov). The other differences between left and right targets were not statistically significant: P>0.05

With respect to single-unit data, it was found that some auditory cortical neurons exhibited a clear influence of the behavioral context, both on their discharge rate and temporal response pattern. For instance, the response to the tone instructing a given movement (instruction stimulus, IS) was different from the response to the same tone delivered when the monkey reached the target, thus confirming that the behavioral response was correct (confirmation stimulus, CS). It should be pointed out here that the IS and the CS were delivered one after the other in the same trials, separated by a delay ranging from approximately 1,500 to 2,800 ms, including the “waiting period” preceding the go-signal, the RT and the motor response time. The neuron illustrated in Fig. 9 exhibited a dramatic difference between the activity related to the IS versus to the CS. A statistically significant On response was observed when the trials were aligned to the onset of the IS, a response that did not appear when the same trials were aligned to the onset of the same stimulus presented as CS. Such an effect (IS versus CS) was observed in 33.2% of neurons tested for the behavioral paradigm. Responses latencies to the IS and CS were compared and did not shown statistically significant differences. In details, for On responses, the mean latency to the IS was 30 ms (SD 10 ms) whereas it was 31.3 ms (SD 4.3 ms) for the CS. For Off responses, the mean latencies were 23.8 (SD 7.5 ms) and 21.1 ms (SD 6.1 ms), following the offset of the IS and CS, respectively.
Fig. 9.

PSTHs (bin width 40 ms) and dot rasters illustrate the responses of a single auditory cortical cell to a pure tone at BF (200 Hz), presented at 60 dB SPL either as instructing stimulus (IS, top panel) or as confirmation stimulus (CS, bottom panel). The same trials are depicted in the two panels, but they were aligned on the IS in the top panel and on the CS in the bottom panel. The onset of stimulus is indicated by the thin vertical line at time zero. In the dot rasters, the second thin vertical line indicates stimulus offset, at time 0.5 s. In the PSTHs, the solid horizontal line represents the mean activity of reference and the dashed horizontal line, a deviation from the mean activity of reference of +3 SDs (see Methods). Discrete events in the task are represented by different symbols in the dot rasters. Note the presence of an On response to the IS (top panel), which does not appear when the same stimulus was presented as CS (bottom panel)

As mentioned earlier, the monkey reached a level of performance higher than 90% and therefore relatively few errors were made (pointing the wrong target). However, in rare cases, the animal made more erroneous trials than usual, offering the possibility to compare the response to the IS between correct and erroneous trials. For some units (3.7%) there was a statistically significant difference between the two activities. The neuron shown in Fig. 10 exhibited an On–Off response to an IS (1,200 Hz) delivered in trials where the monkey performed a correct left-target touch (Fig. 10, top panel). In contrast, the same stimulus presented as IS did not elicit a clear response in trials where the monkey pointed incorrectly to the right target (Fig. 10, bottom panel).
Fig. 10.

PSTHs (bin width 40 ms) and dot rasters illustrate the responses of a single auditory cortical cell to a pure tone at 1,200 Hz (60 dB SPL), presented as instruction stimulus (IS) and instructing a left target touch, for correct trials (top panel) or for incorrect trials (bottom panel). Trials were aligned on the IS in the two panels. The onset of stimulus is indicated by the thin vertical line at time zero. The second thin vertical line indicates stimulus offset, at time 0.5 s. In PSTHs, the solid horizontal line represents the mean activity of reference and the dashed horizontal line a deviation from the mean activity of reference of +3 SDs (see Methods). Discrete events in the task are represented by different symbols in the dot rasters. Note an On–Off response to the IS in the top panel, absent or dramatically reduced in the bottom panel, although the stimulus was the same. Note in the upper PSTHs the presence of several significant bins later during the trials, corresponding to a possible activation of the unit in response to the subsequent presentation of the same stimulus as confirmation (CS) and/or delivery of reward. In the lower PSTH, the presence of several significant bins later during the trials are also due to the presentation, in this case in MK1, of an acoustic error-signal (see Methods) and/or related to the delivery of reward

Discussion

To reduce the duration of the protocol of stimulation and to put emphasis on the behavioral acousticomotor paradigm, the determination of the FRF was shortened, limited to an approximation based on 11 tone frequencies presented at 5 intensity levels. As a result, we did not establish quantitatively the frequency tuning curves given by iso-rate contours, as done by Recanzone et al. (Recanzone 2000b; Recanzone et al. 2000a). Instead, we obtained a qualitative approximation of the FRF, sufficient, however, to categorize units in 3 groups (Fig. 7) and assess the variability of response patterns within the FRF, an issue rarely addressed so far in the auditory cortex of the awake macaque monkey. As a result, a direct comparison with previous studies concerning frequency selectivity is therefore not straightforward. Nevertheless, qualitatively, the three groups of FRFs reported here (Fig. 7) also appear in the figures illustrating the frequency response areas determined on the basis of discharge rate (Schwartz and Tomlinson 1990; Recanzone 2000b; Recanzone et al. 2000a). Due to our protocol compromise, we did not establish systematically the threshold at BF and therefore our data do not allow a comparison of frequency tuning with studies where this parameter was established quantitatively with Q10 or Q40 values (Pelleg-Toiba and Wollberg 1989; Recanzone et al. 2000a).

The patterns of response to tone bursts reported here (On, Off, On–Off, sustained, and inhibition) were observed in the awake squirrel monkey exposed to passive tonal stimulation (Funkenstein and Winter 1973; Pelleg-Toiba and Wollberg 1989) and in the awake macaque monkey performing a simple reaction-time auditory task (Pfingst and O’Connor 1981). Response patterns to tone and noise bursts in the auditory cortex of primate were compared with patterns defined in the cochlear nucleus (Recanzone 2000a). If some response patterns were common for the two structures (On, sustained), responses classified as “primary-like” or “chopper” in the cochlear nucleus were not seen in the auditory cortex (Recanzone 2000a; present study). On the other hand, Off responses were frequently observed in the auditory cortex of primate but not in the cochlear nucleus (Recanzone 2000a; present study). The attribution of a response pattern to a unit is unequivocal in the cochlear nucleus, by taking as criterion the response profile observed at characteristic frequency (CF) 10 dB above threshold (Pfeiffer and Kiang 1965; Pfeiffer 1966). In the auditory cortex, the application of such criterion is less reliable in the sense that the BF may be difficult to define, for instance in units exhibiting multiple peaks of sensitivity in their FRF (e.g., our FRF groups I and III). After eliminating ambiguous cases, we tentatively attributed a response pattern to a unit, defined as the response profile observed at BF for the lowest intensity tested generating a response. Recanzone (2000a) classified the units in two main classes, “tonic” and “phasic,” representing 30–40% and 60–70% of units in the areas A1 and the caudomedial area (CM), respectively. One may tentatively consider that the response patterns sustained and inhibition observed in the present report would roughly correspond to the class “tonic” and would thus amount to 12%. How to explain the lowest proportion of tonically responsive units in the present study? First, the tone bursts used by Recanzone (2000a) were much shorter (200 ms) than in the present study (500 ms). Units responding for approximately 200 ms and classified as tonic by Recanzone (2000a) may become phasic if the stimulus lasts longer. In addition, the criteria used to define a tonic unit are not the same in the two studies. Finally, our definition of response patterns based on visual inspection of dot rasters and PSTHs may contribute to underestimate the proportions of tonic Inhibitions, particularly when the spontaneous discharge rate is low, a property relatively frequently exhibited by the units included in the present analysis.

A major conclusion of the present report is the variability of response patterns to tones observed for a given unit within its FRF. Several units illustrated in the present report displayed a progressive change of response pattern when varying stimulus parameters (frequency and/or intensity; see Figs. 4, 5, 6, 8). Most units exhibited a FRF classified as group I (multiple peaks) and, among them, the majority included three different response patterns in their FRF (Table 1). Overall, 68% of units displayed at least one change of response pattern in the FRF, a figure fairly comparable with the proportion of approximately 60% reported by Pelleg-Toiba and Wollberg (1989) in the squirrel monkey. The observation that a majority of units changed their response patterns to tones within their FRF emphasizes the notion that this parameter does not allow an unequivocal classification of units in the auditory cortex of awake and behaving monkeys. Furthermore, in agreement with the conclusions of Recanzone (2000a), the frequent and progressive changes of response patterns observed here in the FRFs argue for a continuum from one response pattern to another rather than for strictly separated categories.

The behavioral acousticomotor association task described here is reminiscent of the paradigm reported by Vaadia et al. (1982). These authors showed two examples of units in the auditory cortex exhibiting a variation of response to tone (1 unit) or to noise (the other unit) when the same stimulus instructed first a left and then a right shift of a lever using the arm. The proportion of units exhibiting this behavioral meaning effect was 17% (Vaadia et al. 1982). Such units reflecting the processing of acousticomotor association were also observed in our paradigm, but they will be described in details in a separate report. As an original contribution with respect to the study of Vaadia et al. (1982), the present report provides evidence that the same stimulus can produce a different response when presented as “instruction cue” or as “confirmation stimulus” (Fig. 9). Such an observation reflecting a context effect, observed in approximately one-third of units, can be interpreted in different ways: (1) a more prominent response to the same stimulus when presented as IS than as CS may reflect a more focused attention of the monkey to the IS than to the CS. Indeed, the IS is crucial in order to select the correct movement, whereas the CS is presented after the behavioral response and therefore cannot influence the performance; (2) a certain level of attention (different to that focused on the IS) may be associated to the CS, because the latter informs the monkey whether its motor selection was correct or not. In other words, in contrast to the IS, the CS includes a component of positive reinforcement; (3) the sensory response to the CS may be influenced by the fact that it is a stimulus that was already presented earlier during the trial. Indeed, it was shown that a forward masking or enhancement effect due to a preceding stimulus can influence the second response (Brosch and Schreiner 1997; Brosch et al. 1999); however, the absence of response to the CS in Fig. 9 is unlikely to be due to such a forward masking, because of the relatively long delay between the IS and the CS (at least 1.5 s). Another possible source of suppression of response to the second stimulus is adaptation, corresponding to a decrease in responsiveness to repeated or prolonged stimulation, thus becoming behaviorally irrelevant. In the present paradigm, the IS and CS are neither prolonged nor irrelevant, thus excluding such dramatic adaptation effect on the second response; (4) in contrast to the IS, the CS “predicts” the delivery of a reward, a condition known to influence the discharge properties of neurons in the basal ganglia, prefrontal, and cingulate cortices (see Schultz et al. 2000; Hikosaka and Watanabe 2000 for review; Shima and Tanji 1998). Whether responses in the auditory cortex can be modulated by the expectation of a reward remains to be determined by designing specific behavioral paradigms focused on the reward delivery.

In absence of monitoring of movements of the eyes, one cannot completely exclude that some of the effects observed here in the auditory cortex may derive from an influence of eye position. Indeed, an effect of eye position has been recently reported for approximately 20% of neurons in the core area of the auditory cortex of macaque monkeys (Werner-Reiss et al. 2003). However, the proportion of responding units was higher than 20% in the present study. Furthermore, systematic and precisely time-locked onset or offset responses to the IS and CS with short latencies (20–30 ms on average) as observed here can hardly be produced by systematic changes in eye position locked to the acoustic stimulus. Indeed, in the present experimental conditions, the acoustic stimulus was not delivered at a restricted position of the acoustic space to which the animal may have produced a saccade. Moreover, signals coming from the frontal eye field or from other oculomotor centers are likely to take more time than 20–30 ms to reach the auditory cortex.

Also original, as far as we know, is the observation of auditory cortical neurons whose responses to an instructing tonal stimulus varied significantly depending on whether the monkey performed correctly or not the acousticomotor association (Fig. 10). In a way, these units seem to “predict,” at the level of single cell in the auditory cortex, the motor behavior of the animal. Due to the rarity of erroneous trials after intensive training of the animals, opportunities to collect enough incorrect trials are few and therefore it was not possible to infer the proportion of such “predicting” units in the auditory cortex, without making an underestimation. Of course, this type of units is likely to be encountered fairly frequently in the prefrontal cortex, in particular in areas involved in the selection of the motor response. The presence of such units early in the acousticomotor loop, at the level of the auditory cortex, remains to be explained. It may well represent the result of a feedback information from the prefrontal cortex to the auditory cortex. The anatomical support of this feedback influence may be the dense projections, demonstrated anatomically, from areas 10, 12, 45, and 46 of the prefrontal cortex and terminating in the belt and parabelt regions of the auditory cortex (Romanski et al. 1999a, 1999b).

Acknowledgements

The authors thank V. Moret, F. Tinguely, and C. Roulin for histology; A. Gaillard, B. Aebischer, and E. Regli for designing and building the mechanics and electronics of home-made equipment as well as maintenance; J. Corpataux and B. Morandi for taking care of the animals in the animal room; L. Monney for informatics. The work was supported financially by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant 31-61857.00) and the National Center for Competence in Research (NCCR, Neural Plasticity and Repair). We thank Dr. Hoogewoud and Mr. Devaud (Cantonal Hospital of Fribourg) for the collection of CT scan data.

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2003