Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of gender and time-of-day on time perception and mental workload

Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments are reported which investigated how subject gender and time-of-day influenced the estimation of duration and the perception of task-related mental workload. In the first experiment, 24 subjects performed a filled time-estimation task in a constant blacked-out, noise-reduced environment at 0800h, 1200h, 1600h, and 2000h, respectively. In the second experiment, 12 different subjects performed an unfilled time estimation task in similar conditions at 0900h, 1400h, and 1900h. At the termination of all experimental sessions, participants completed the NASA Task Load Index workload assessment questionnaire as a measure of perceived mental workload. Results indicated that physiological response, reflected in body temperature change, followed an expected pattern of sequential increase with time-of-day. However, estimates of duration and the perception of mental workload showed no significant effects for time-of-day. In each of the experiments there were significant differences in time estimation and mental workload response contingent on the gender of the participant. These results are interpreted in light of the previous positive findings for circadian fluctuation in performance efficiency and the equivocal findings of a gender difference in time estimation. A unifying account of these collective results is given based on gender by time-of-day interactional effects.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adams, J.A. (1964). Motor skills.Annual Review of Psychology, 202, 181–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adkins, S. (1964). Performance, heart rate, and respiration rate on the day-night continuum.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 18, 409–412.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asso, D. (1987). Cyclical variations. In M.A. Baker (Ed.),Sex differences in human performance. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axel, R, (1924). Estimation of time.Archives of Psychology, 12 No. 74.

  • Bakan, P., & Kleba, F. (1957). Reliability of time estimates.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 7, 23–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M.A. (1987). (Ed.).Sex differences in human performance. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M.A., Holding, D.H., & Loeb, M. (1984). Noise, sex and time-of-day effects in a mathematics task.Ergonomics, 27, 67–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M.A., & Pangburn, K. (1982). Female temperature cycles and their relationship to performance. Paper presented at the 20th Congress of the International Association of Applied Psychology, Edinburgh, Scotland.

  • Baker, M.A., Quinkert, K., Holding, D.H., & Colquhoun, W.P. (March, 1989).Time of day and female temperature as factors in performance. Southern Society for Philosophy and Psychology, New Orleans, LA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, R.O., Thor, D.H., & Wright, D.E. (1966). Sex differences in the sense of time: Failure to replicate a 1904 study.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 22, 398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, C.R. (1972). Accurate performance of a time estimation task in relation to sex, age, and personality variables.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 175–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergson, H. (1910).Time and free will. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bindra, D., & Waksberg, H. (1956). Methods and terminology in studies of time estimation.Psychological Bulletin, 53, 155–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, V., & Feinberg, I. (1970). Time judgment as a function of method, practice, and sex.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 85, 171–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chignell, M.H., & Hancock, P.A. (1985). Knowledge-based load leveling and task allocation in human-machine systems.Proceedings of the Annual Conference on Manual Control, 21, 9.1–9.11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christie, M.J., & McBrearty, E.M.T. (1977). Deep body temperature: Diurnal variation, sex and personality.Journal of Psychosomatic Research.20, 501–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christie, M.J., & McBrearty, E.M.T. (1979). Psychophysiological investigations of post lunch state in male and female subjects.Ergonomics, 22, 307–323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colquhoun, W.P. (1971). Circadian variations in mental efficiency. In W.P. Colquhoun (Ed.),Biological rhythms and human performance. (pp. 39–107). Academic Press: London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damos, D.L. (1988). Individual differences in subjective estimates of workload. In P.A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.).Human mental workload. (pp. 231–237). North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damos, D.L., & Lyall, E.A. (1986). The effect of varying stimulus and response modes and asymmetric transfer on the dual-task performance of discrete tasks.Ergonomics, 29, 519–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delay, E.R., & Richardson, M.A. (1981). Time estimation in humans; Effects of ambient illumination and sex.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 747–750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derrick, W.L. (1988). Dimensions of operator workload.Human Factors, 30, 95–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dresslar, F.B. (1892). Some influences which affect the rapidity of voluntary movements.American Journal of Psychology, 4, 514–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelstein, B.C. (1982). The efficacy of ear canal temperature as an indirect measure of cerebral blood flow in non-stressful and stressful conditions. Doctoral dissertation. California School of Professional Psychology, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Empson, J. (1977). Periodicity in body temperature in man.Experientia, 33, 342–343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folkard, S., & Monk, T.H., (1979). Time-of-day and processing strategy in free recall.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 461–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folkard, S., & Monk, T.H. (1980). Circadian rhythms in human memory.British Journal of Psychology, 71, 295–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Folkard, S., Weaver, R., & Wildgruber, C. (1983). Multi-oscillatory control of circadian rhythms in human performance.Nature, 305. 223–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Francois, M. (1927). Contribution a l’etude du sens du temps. La temperature interne comme facteur de l’appreciation subjective des durees.Annee Psychologie.28, 186–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J.H., Platt, P.E, & Singer, M. (1964). A sex difference in time estimation.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19, 42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Getsinger, S.H. (1974). Temporal estimation, sex, and ego strength.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 322.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J., & Allan, L. (Eds.). (1984).Timing and time perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, No. 423.

  • Gilliland, A.R., Hofeld, J., & Eckstrand, G. (1946). Studies in time perception.Psychological Bulletin, 43, 162–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilliland, A.R., & Humphreys, D.W. (1943). Age, sex, method and interval as variables in time estimation.Journal of General Psychology, 63, 123–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, S. (1968). Variability of temporal judgement: Intersensory comparisons and sex differences.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 211–215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopher, D., & Donchin, E. (1986). Workload: An examination of the concept. In K. Boff, L. Kaufman., & J.P. Thomas, (Eds.).Handbook of perception and human performance. (pp. 1–49). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenburg, R., & Kurz, R. (1968). Influence of type of stressor and sex of subject on time estimation.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 899–903.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guay, M., & Hall, C. (1977). Human time estimation: Bibliography.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 1087–1092.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gulliksen, H. (1927). The influence of occupation upon the perception of time.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 52–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guyau, J.M. (1890).La genese de l’idee de temps. Paris: Alcan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P.A. (1983). The effect of an induced selective increase in head temperature upon performance of a simple mental task.Human Factors, 25, 441–448.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P.A. (1989). The effect of performance failure and task demand on the perception of mental workload.Applied Ergonomics, 20, 197–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P.A. (1992).The internal clock. Manuscript in preparation.

  • Hancock, P.A., Chignell, M.H., & Kerr, G. (August 1988). Defining task complexity and task difficulty.Proceedings of the XXIV International Congress of Psychology, Sydney, Australia.

  • Hancock, P.A., & Meshkati, N. (Eds.) (1988).Human mental workload, North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P.A., Meshkati, N., & Robertson, M.M. (1985). Physiological reflections of mental workload.Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 56, 1110–1114.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P.A., & Vercruyssen, M. (1992).Sex-related differences in time perception. Manuscript in preparation.

  • Hancock, P.A., & Warm, J.S. (1989). A dynamic model of stress and sustained attention.Human Factors, 31, 519–537.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, S.G. (1988). Personal communication.

  • Hart, S.G., & Staveland, L.E. (1988). Development of NASA TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. In P.A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.).Human mental workload. (pp. 139–183) North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoagland, H. (1933). The physiological control of judgements of duration: Evidence for a chemical clock.Journal of General Psychology, 9, 267–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, J.A., & Coyne, I. (1975). Seasonal changes in the circadian variation of oral temperature during wakefulness.Experientia, 31, 1296–1297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hornstein, A., & Rotter, G. (1969). Research methodology in temporal perception.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 79, 561–564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huyck, M.H. (1990). Gender differences in aging. In J.E. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.).Handbook of the psychology of aging. (3rd Edition), (pp. 124–132). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M.R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory.Psychological Review, 83, 323–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kantowitz, B.H. (1987). Mental workload. In P.A. Hancock (Ed.).Human factors psychology (pp. 81–121). North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirkcaldy, B.D. (1984). Individual differences in time estimation.International Journal of Sport Psychology, 15, 11–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleitman, N. (1939/1963).Sleep and wakefulness. University of Chicago Press: Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., & Zakay, D. (Eds.). (1988).Time and human cognition: A life span perspective. North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J.C., (1959). The influence of different activities on the apparent length of time.Psychological Monographs, 73, Whole No. 474.

  • MacDougall, R. (1904). Sex differences in the sense of time.Science, 19, 707–708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, G.A., Shumate, M., & Frauenfelder, K. (1981). Experience of duration as a function of the number of responses, task difficulty, and sex.Perceptual and Motor Skills.53, 139–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCauley, M.E., Kennedy, R.S., & Bittner, A.C. Jr. (1980). Development of performance evaluation tests for environmental research (PETER): Time estimation.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 655–665.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J.D. (1979). Variations in perception of short time intervals during menstrual cycle.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 940–942.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Montare, A. (1985). Learning effects of knowledge of results upon time estimation.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 871–877.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, S.E. (1982).Estimates of sleep latency and of other temporal intervals in insomniac and normal sleepers. Masters thesis, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore-Ede, M.C., Sulzman, F.M., & Fuller, C.A. (1982).The clocks that time us. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moray, N. (1979). (Ed.).Mental workload: Its theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, R.D., & Eggemeier, F.T. (1986). Workload assessment methodology. In K. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J.P. Thomas, (Eds.).Handbook of perception and human performance. (pp. 1–49). Wiley: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, R.E. (1969).On the experience of time. Baltimore: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, J.E. (1980).The psychobiology of sex differences and sex roles. New York: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfaff, D. (1968). Effects of temperature and time-of-day on time judgments.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 419–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pöppel, E. (1978). Time perception. In R. Held, H. Leibowitz, and H-L. Teuber (Eds.).Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Vol VIII (pp. 713–729). Berlin.

  • Poppel, E., & Giedke, H. (1970). Diurnal variation of time perception.Psychologie Forschung, 34, 182–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulton, E.C. (1982). Influential companions: Effects of one strategy on another in the within-subject designs of cognitive psychology.Psychological Bulletin, 92, 673–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinkert, K. (1985).A multifactor approach to the study of circadian rhythms in temperature and skill performance. Doctoral dissertation, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rammsayer, T., & Lustnauer, S. (1989), Sex differences in time perception.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 195–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roeckelein, V.E. (1972). Sex differences in time estimation.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 859–862.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smythe, E., & Goldstone, S. (1957). The time sense: A normative genetic study of the development of time perception.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 7, 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swift, E.Y., & McGeoch, J.A. (1925). An experimental study of the perception of filled and empty time.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8, 240–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavris, C., & Wade, C. (1984).The longest war: Sex differences in perspective. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thor, D.H. (1962). Diurnal variability in time perception.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 15, 451–454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thor, D.H., & Baldwin, R.O. (1965). Time-of-day estimates at six times of day under normal conditions.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 21, 904–906.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thor, D.H., & Crawford, M.L.J. (1964). Time perception during a two-week confinement: Influence of age, sex, IQ, and time of day.Acta Psychologica, 22, 78–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the internal clock.Psychological Monographs, 77, (Whole No. 576).

  • Treisman, M. (1984). Temporal rhythms and cerebral rhythms.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 542–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Unger, R.K. (1979).Female and male: Psychological perspectives. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vidulich, M.A. (1988). The cognitive psychology of subjective mental workload. In P.A. Hancock and N. Meshkati (Eds.).Human mental workload. (pp. 219–229). North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webb, W.B. (Ed.). (1982).Biological rhythms, sleep, and performance. Wiley: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wever, R.A. (1984). Sex differences in human circadian rhythms: Intrinsic periods and sleep fractions.Experientia, 40, 1226–1234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G.F. & O’Donnell, R.D. (1988). Measurement of operator workload with the neuropsychological workload test battery. In P.A. Hancock & N. Meshkati (Eds.).Human mental workload. (pp. 63–100). North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yeh, Y.Y., & Wickens, C.D. (1988). Dissociation of performance and subjective measures of workload.Human Factors, 30, 111–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R., & Urban, F. (1906). Time estimation in its relation to sex, age, and physiological rhythms.Harvard Psychological Studies, 405–430.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hancock, P.A., Vercruyssen, M. & Rodenburg, G.J. The effect of gender and time-of-day on time perception and mental workload. Current Psychology 11, 203–225 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02686841

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02686841

Keywords

Navigation