Skip to main content
Log in

Developmental changes in character-complexity and word-length effects when reading Chinese script

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study was designed to examine the developmental changes in the character-complexity and word-length effects when reading Chinese script. Character complexity was defined in two ways: (1) the number of constituent strokes for characters (Experiment 1), and (2) the number of constituent radicals for characters (Experiment 2). The word length was defined as the number of characters in words (Experiment 3). The three experiments involved a lexical recognition task, and the participants consisted of 25 second graders, 24 fourth graders, 24 sixth graders, and 25 university students. In Experiment 1, it was found that the response latencies increased with the number of strokes in characters for second graders, whereas no effects were evident for fourth graders, sixth graders, or university students. In contrast, in Experiment 2 no character-complexity effect was found for the number of radicals. In Experiment 3, only a partial word-length effect was found for the number of characters for second graders. These results suggest that beginning Chinese readers process characters in an analytical way, but that the decoding process changes gradually from analytic to holistic as their reading skills develop. The educational implication of this result is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aghababian, V., & Nazir, T. (2000). Developing normal reading skills: Aspects of the visual processes underlying word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 123–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bijeljac-Babic, R., Millogo, V., Farioli, F., & Grainger, J. (2004). A developmental investigation of word length effects in reading using a new on-line word identification paradigm. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 411–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, L. K., Hess, U., & Wolford, G. (1993). Transformations for within-subject designs: A Monte Carlo investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 566–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y. P., Allport, D. A., & Marshall, J. C. (1996). What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: The stroke or the stroke pattern? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 1024–1043.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H.-C., & Cho, S.-L. (2000). 認知心理學實驗週邊組件的設計 [Peripheral equipment design for cognitive experiments]. 應用心理學報 [Chinese Journal of Applied Psychology], 8, 41-60.

  • Chen, S.-C., & Liu, I.-M. (2000). Functional orthographic units in Chinese character recognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 32(supplement), 13–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, J.-L., & Su, Y.-F. (2009, in press). 國小不同認字能力學童辨識中文字詞之字元複雜度效果與詞長效果研究 [The effect of decoding ability on Character-Complexity and Word-length effects in Taiwanese beginning readers]. 教育心理學報 [Bulletin of Educational Psychology].

  • Cheng, C. M. (1981). 漢字的認知歷程 [Perception of Chinese characters]. 中華心理學刊 [Acta Psychologia Taiwanica], 23, 137–154.

  • Cheung, H., & Ng, L. K. H. (2003). Chinese reading development in some major Chinese societies: An introduction. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 3–17). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiang, W.-C. (2003). The basic orthographic units in children’s recognition of Chinese characters. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 45(1), 47–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinese Knowledge Information Processing Group. (1993). 新聞語料詞頻統計表:語料庫研究系列之二 [Corpus-based frequency count of words in journal Chinese: Corpus-based research series no. 2]. Taipei, Taiwan: Institute of Information Science Academia Sinica.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Borrelli, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Reading words and pseudo-words: An eye movement study of developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 80, 617–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (1999). Eye movement patterns in linguistic and non-linguistic tasks in developmental surface dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1407–1420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, K. Y., Gao, J. Y., & Ao, X. P. (1984). Pronunciation principles of the Chinese character and alphabetic writing scripts. Chinese character reform, 3 (pp. 19–22). Beijing: National Commission of Chinese Character Reform.

  • Fang, S.-P. (1994). English word length effects and the Chinese character-word difference: Truth or myth? Chinese Journal of Psychology, 36(1), 59–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fang, S.-P. (2003). Are there differential word length effects in the two visual fields? Brain and Language, 85, 467–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of Chinese characters. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 559–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, L. (2000). Reading aloud polysyllabic words and nonwords: The syllabic length effect reexamined. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 142–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. (2002). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the mind. New York, NY: Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Ng, T.-T., & Ng, W.-K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 849–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Honorof, D. N., & Feldman, L. (2006). The Chinese character in psycholinguistic research: Form, structure, and the reader. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), The handbook of east Asian psycholinguistics: Vol. 1. Chinese (pp. 195–208). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H.-S. (2001). 中文年級認字量表 [Graded Chinese character recognition test]. Taipei, Taiwan: Psychological Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hue, C. W., & Tzeng, A. K. (2000). Lexical recognition task: A new method for the study of Chinese character recognition. Acta Psychologia Sinica, 32, 60–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). Naming multisyllabic words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 92–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juphard, A., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (2004). Length effect in reading and lexical decision: Evidence from skilled readers and a developmental dyslexic participant. Brain and Cognition, 55, 332–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1987). Orthography: Its structure and effects on reading. In M. A. Just & P. A. Carpenter (Eds.), The psychology of reading and language processing (pp. 287–325). Newton, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leck, K. J., Weekes, B. S., & Chen, M. J. (1995). Visual and phonological pathways to the lexicon: Evidence from Chinese readers. Memory and Cognition, 23, 468–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K., Cheng, P.-W., & Mulcahy, R. (1987). Automatic processing of morphemic orthography by mature readers. Language and Speech, 30(2), 181–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. Brain and Language, 98, 140–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute for Compilation and Translation. (1999). 國民小學常用字彙研究字頻總表 [A frequency count of primary school reading material]. Taipei, Taiwan: National Institute for Compilation and Translation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nazir, T., Ben-Boutayab, N., Decoppet, N., Deutsch, A., & Frost, R. (2004). Reading habits, perceptual learning, and the recognition of printed words. Brain and Language, 88, 294–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English lexicon project. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 45–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2006). Reading Chinese characters: Orthography, phonology, meaning, and the lexical constituency model. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), The handbook of east Asian psycholinguistics: Vol. 1. Chinese (pp. 225–236). NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1999). The constituency model of Chinese word identification. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 115–134). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J. (2002). Reading fluency: Its development and assessment. In A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction (pp. 166–183). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuels, S. J., LaBerge, D., & Bremer, C. D. (1978). Units of word recognition: Evidence for developmental changes. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 715–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Su, Y.-F. (1997). Indicators of automaticity in word recognition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities.

  • Sun, F., & Feng, D. (1999). Eye movements in reading Chinese and English text. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 189–205). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1997). Submorphemic processing in Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 761–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., & Peng, D.-L. (1990). Yujing dui hanyu danzici tezheng fenxi de yingxiang [The effects of semantic context on the feature analyses of single Chinese characters]. Xin li xue dong tai [Journal of Psychology], 4, 5–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Leij, A., & van Daal, V. H. P. (1999). Automatization aspects of dyslexia: Speed limitations in word identification, sensitivity to increasing task demands, and orthographic compensation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 417–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 439–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, H.-M. (1998). 閱讀障礙學生文字辨識自動化處理之分析研究 [An analysis of automatic processing of word recognition in dyslexics]. 特殊教育與復健學報 [Bulletin of Special Education and Rehabilitation], 6, 15–37.

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Korne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 93, 369–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Part of this research was supported by grant no. NSC89-2413-H-003-033-F18 from the National Science Council, Taiwan. We are grateful to two anonymous reviewers for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We also thank Dr. Yi-Chen Wu, Ms. Hsiou-Wen Yang, and Mr. Pang-Tsung Chien for valuable assistance in data collection, Dr. Zhong-Fan Hu for computer programming, and the Academic Paper Editing Clinic, National Taiwan Normal University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yi-Fen Su.

Appendix

Appendix

See Tables 4, 5 and 6.

Table 4 The number of strokes and frequencies of low-, moderate-, and high-complexity characters used in Experiment 1
Table 5 The number of strokes and frequencies of two-, three-, and four-radical characters used in Experiment 2
Table 6 The frequency of two-, three-, and four-character words used in Experiment 3

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Su, YF., Samuels, S.J. Developmental changes in character-complexity and word-length effects when reading Chinese script. Read Writ 23, 1085–1108 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9197-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9197-3

Keywords

Navigation