Skip to main content
Log in

Reading–writing connections: Discourse-oriented research

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

As reading and writing are both language processes, one can assume relationships between them, but the exact nature of these relationships has not yet been determined. While a large body of research has addressed reading comprehension and written production independently, very little investigation has examined the possible relationships between these two psycholinguistic processes, particularly from a discourse and cognitive perspective. Argumentative texts were analyzed in the present investigation on reading–writing connections. Four tests were designed and tested on 439 eighth graders. The tests assessed psycholinguistic variables that account for the microstructural, macrostructural and superstructural levels of comprehension/production processing. Correlation results showed significant coefficients between reading and writing of argumentative texts in all the psycholinguistic levels analyzed. These results suggest that the processes involved in both activities share some common knowledge-based strategies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arnoux E., Nogueira S., Silvestri A. (2002) La construcción de representaciones enunciativas: El reconocimiento de voces en la comprensión de textos polifónicos [The construction of representations: Voice recognition in reading poliphonic texts]. Revista Signos [Signs Journal] 35: 129–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Belanger J. (1987) Theory and research into reading and writing connections: A critical review. Reading-Canada Lecture 5: 10–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Boscolo P., Cisotto L. (1999) On narrative reading–writing relationships: How young writers construe the reader’s need for inferences. In: Goldman S., Graesser A., van den Broek P. (Eds) Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence. Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 161–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Brem S., Russell J., Weems L. (2001) Science on the web: Students’ evaluations of scientific arguments. Discourse Processes 32: 191–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camps A. (1995). Aprender a escribir textos argumentativos: Características dialógicas de la argumentación escrita [Learning to write argumentative texts: Dialogic characteristics in composing argumentation]. Infancia y Aprendizaje [Childhood and learning] 25: 51–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Camps A., Millian M. (2000) Metalinguistic activity in learning to write. Amsterdan University Press, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Coirier P., Gaonac’h D., Passerault J. (1996) Psycholinguistique textuelle [Text psycholinguistics]. Hachette, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper L., Odell R. (1977) Evaluating writing: Describing, measuring, judging. NCTE, Urbana, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper L., Odell R. (1998) Evaluating writing: The role of teacher´s knowledge about text, learning & culture. NCTE, Urbana, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Crombach L. (1988) Five perspectives on validity argument. In: Wainer H., Braun H. (Eds) Test validity. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 3–19

    Google Scholar 

  • de Beaugrande R. (1997) New foundations for a science of text and discourse: Cognition, communication, and the freedom of access to knowledge and society. Ablex, Norwood, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckhoff S. (1983). How reading affects children’s writing. Language Arts 60: 234–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Ede L., Lundford A. (1988) Audience addressed/audience invoked: The role of audience in composition theory and pedagogy. In: Tate G., Corbett E. (Eds) The writing teacher’s sourcebook. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 248–259

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisterhold J. (1991) Reading–writing connections: Toward a description for second language learners. In: Kroll B. (Ed) Second language writing: Research insights for the classroom. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 88–101

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton M., Kuhn D. (2001) The development of argumentative discourse skill. Discourse Processes 32: 135–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald J., Shanahan T. (2000) Reading and writing relations and their development. Educational Psychologist 35: 39–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher M. (1990) Language comprehension as structure building. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Golder C., Coirier P. (1994) Argumentative text writing: Developmental trends. Discourse Processes 18: 187–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez L. (1994) La dimensión social de la comprensión textual [Social dimension of textual comprehension]. Revista Signos [Signs Journal] 27: 35–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser A., Bower G. (1992) Inferences and text comprehension. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser A., Gernsbacher M., Goldman S. (1997) Cognition. In: Van Dijk T. (Ed) Discourse as structure and process. Discourse studies: A multidisciplinary introduction Vol. 1. SAGE, London, pp. 7–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser A., Magliano J., Haberlandt K. (1994a) Psychological studies of naturalistic text. In: van Oostendorp H., Zwaan R. (Eds) Naturalistic text comprehension. Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 9–33

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser A., Singer M., Trabasso T. (1994b) Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review 101: 371–395

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser A., Swamer S., Baggett W., Sell M. (1996) New models of deep comprehension. In: Britton B., Graesser A. (Eds) Models of understanding text. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 5–27

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene S., Ackerman J. (1995) Expanding the constructivist metaphor: A rethorical perspective on literacy research and practice. Review of Educational Research 65: 383–420

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair J., Anderson R., Tatham J., Black W. (1999) Multivariate data analysis. Prentice Hall, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Harp B., Brewer J. (1991) Reading and writing: Teaching for the connections. Harcourt Brace Yovanovich, Orlando, FL

    Google Scholar 

  • Hass A. (1989) Collaboration through writing and reading. NCTE, Urbana, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedges L. (1988) The meta-analysis of test validity studies: Some new approaches. In: Wainer H., Braun H. (Eds) Test validity. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 191–213

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller M. (1995) Reading–writing connections: From theory to practice. Longman, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin J. (1992) Research in reading/writing from 1900 to 1984. In: Irwin J., Doyle M. (Eds) Reading/writing connections: Learning from research. IRA, Newark, DE, pp. 308–333

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, J., & Doyle, M. (Eds.) (1992). Reading/writing connections: Learning from research. Newark, Delaware: IRA

  • Karmiloff-Smith A. (1992) Beyond modulary: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Kent, T. (Ed.) (1999). Post-process theory. Beyond the writing-process paradigm. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press

  • Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction–integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch W. (1998) Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucer S. (1985) The making of meaning: Reading and writing as parallel processes. Written Communication 2: 317–356

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucer S. (2001) Dimensions of literacy. A conceptual base for teaching reading and writing in school settings. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer J. (1986) Children reading and writing. Structures and strategies. Ablex Publishing, Norwood, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer J. (2002) Effective literacy instruction. Building successful reading and writing programs. NCTE, Urbana, IL

    Google Scholar 

  • Madden C., Zwaan R. (2003) How does verb aspect constrain event representation? Memory & Cognition 31: 663–672

    Google Scholar 

  • Myers T., Brown K., McGonigle B. (1986) Reasoning and discourse processes. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson N., Calfee R. (1998) The reading–writing connections viewed historically. In: Nelson N., Calfee R. (Eds) The reading–writing connection. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 1–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Nystrand M. (1987) The role of context in written communication. In: Horowitz R., Samuels J. (Eds) Comprehending oral and written language. Academic Press, New York, pp. 197–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Nystrand M. (1990) Sharing words: The effects of readers on developing writers. Written Communication 7: 3–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson G. (1999) Toward a post-process composition: Abandoning the rhetoric of assertion. In: Kent T. (Ed) Post-process theory. Beyond the writing-process paradigm. Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, IL, pp. 7–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi, G. (1989). La inferencia una aproximación al concepto [Inferences: Approaching the concept]. In: Proceedings 8th congress of the Chilean society of linguistics (pp. 211–220). Santiago de Chile

  • Parodi, G. (1990). Los procesos inferenciales en la comprensión de textos escritos [Inferential processes in reading comprehension]. Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso. (Unpublished Masters dissertation)

  • Parodi G. (1992) Estructura textual y estrategias lectoras [Text structure and reading strategies]. Lenguas Modernas [Modern Languages] 19: 89–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi G. (1998) Conexiones entre comprensión y producción de textos escritos: Estudio exploratorio en alumnos de educación básica [Reading–writing connections: Exploratory study with elementary schools students]. Revista Lingüística en el Aula [Journal of Classroom Linguistics] 2: 7–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi G. (2000) La evaluación de a producción de textos escritos argumentativos: Una alternativa cognitivo discursiva [Evaluating written argumentative texts: A discourse and cognitive alternative]. Revista Signos [Signs Journal] 33: 151–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi G. (2001) Comprensión y producción del discurso escrito: Estudio empírico en escolares chilenos [Reading and writing texts: Research with Chilean students]. Revista Iberoamericana de Discurso y Sociedad [Iberoamerican Journal of Discourse and Society] 3: 75–101

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi, G. (2002). Comprensión lingüística: ¿Hacia dónde vamos desde donde estamos? [Reading comprehension: Where do we go from where we are?]. In: G. Parodi (Ed.), Lingüística e interdisciplinariedad: Desafíos del nuevo milenio. Ensayos en honor a Marianne Peronard [Linguistics and interdisciplinarity: Challenges of the new millennium. Essays in honor of Marianne Peronard] (pp. 44–67). Valparaíso: Editorial Universitaria de Valparaíso

  • Parodi G. (2003) Relaciones entre lectura y escritura: Una perspectiva cognitiva discursiva [Relationships between reading and writing: Discursive and cognitive perspectives]. Editorial Universitaria de Valparaíso, Valparaíso

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi G. (2005a) Comprensión de textos escritos [Comprehension of written discourse]. EUDEBA, Buenos, Aires

    Google Scholar 

  • Parodi, G. (Ed.) (2005b). Discurso especializado e instituciones formadoras [Specialized discourse and academic institutions]. Valparaíso: Editorial Universitaria de Valparaíso

  • Parodi, G., & Núñez, P. (1998). El desarrollo de estrategias de lectura comprensiva: Una aplicación experimental del “Programa L y C: Leer y Comprender” [Reading strategies development: An experience with “Programa L y C: Leer y Comprender”]. In: M. Peronard, L. Gómez, G. Parodi, & P. Núñez (Eds.), Comprensión de textos escritos: De la teoría a la sala de clases [Reading comprehension: From theory to practice] (pp. 249–264). Santiago de Chile: Editorial Andrés Bello

  • Parodi, G., & Núñez, P. (1999). En búsqueda de un modelo cognitivo/textual para la evaluación del texto escrito [In search of a model for the evaluation of written texts]. In: M. C. Martínez (Ed.), Comprensión y producción de textos académicos: Expositivos y argumentativos [Reading and writing academic texts: Expositive and argumentative genres] (pp. 81–115). Cali, Colombia: Universidad del Valle

  • Peronard, M., Gómez, L., Parodi, G., & Núñez, P. (1998). Comprensión de textos escritos: De la teoría a la sala de clases [Reading comprehension: From theory to practice]. Santiago de Chile: Editorial Andrés Bello

  • Reuter Y. (1995) Les relations lecture-écriture dans le champ didactique [The reading–writing relationtionship in methodological applications]. Pratiques [Practices] 86: 5–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Rickheit G., Strohner H. (1985) Inferences in text processing. Elsevier Science Publications, Amsterdam, Netherlands

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruth L., Murphy S. (1988) Designing writing tasks for the assessment of writing Ablex, Norwood, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski M., Paivio A. (1994) A dual coding view of imagery and verbal processes in reading comprehension. In: Ruddell R., Rudell M., Singer H. (Eds) Theoretical models and processes of reading. IRA, Newark, DE, pp. 582–601

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski M., Paivio A. (2001) Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Sager M. (1989) Exploiting the reading–writing connection to engage students in text. Journal of Reading 33: 24–39

    Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, I., & álvarez, N. (1999). El discurso argumentativo de los escolares venezolanos: ¿Evolución o estancamiento? [Argumentative discourse in Venezuelan school students: Development or stagnation?]. Paper presented at the 3rd International Congress of Discourse Analysis (ALED): Santiago de Chile

  • Shanahan T. (1984) Nature of the reading–writing relation: An exploratory multivariate analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology 76: 466–477

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanahan T., Lomax R. (1986) An analysis and comparison of theoretical models of the reading–writing relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology 78: 116–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spivey N. (1990) Transforming texts: Constructing processes in reading and writing. Written Communication 7: 256–290

    Google Scholar 

  • Spivey N. (1997) The constructivist metaphor: Reading, writing and the making of meaning. Academic Press, San Diego, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Stotsky S. (1983) Research on reading/writing relationships: A synthesis and suggested directions. Language Arts 60: 627–642

    Google Scholar 

  • Tierney R. (1992) Ongoing research and new directions. In: Irwin J., Doyle M. (Eds) Reading/writing connections: Learning from research. IRA, Newark, DE, pp. 246–259

    Google Scholar 

  • Tierney R., Shanahan T. (1991) Research on the reading–writing relationship: Interactions, transactions, and outcomes. In: Barr R., Kamil M., Mosenthal P., Pearson D. (Eds) Handbook of reading research Vol. II. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 246–280

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk T. (1980) Macrostructures. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hilldale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk T. (1985) Cognitive situation models in discourse production: The expression of ethnic situations in prejudiced discourse. In: Forgas P. (Ed) Language and social situations. Springer Verlag, New York, pp. 61–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk T. (1990) Social cognition and discourse. In: Giles H., Robinson W. P. (Eds) Handbook of language and social psychology. John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp. 145–167

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, T. (1999). ¿Un estudio lingüístico de la ideología? [A linguistic study of ideology?]. In: G. Parodi (Ed.), Discurso, cognición y educación. Ensayos en honor a Luis A. Gómez Macker [Discourse, cognition, and education. Essays in honor of Luis A. Gómez Macker] (pp. 27–42). Valparaíso: Editorial Universitaria de Valparaíso

  • Van Dijk, T. (2002). Tipos de conocimiento en el procesamiento del discurso [Knowledge types in discourse processing]. In: G. Parodi (Ed.), Lingüística e interdisciplinariedad: Desafíos del nuevo milenio. Ensayos en honor a Marianne Peronard [Linguistics and interdisciplinarity: Challenges of the new millennium. Essays in honor of Marianne Peronard] (pp. 30–43). Valparaíso: Editorial Universitaria de Valparaíso

  • Van Dijk T., Kintsch W. (1983) Strategies of discourse comprehension. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Oostendorp H., Zwaan R. (1994) Introduction: Naturalistic texts and naturalistic tasks. In: Van Oostendorp H., Zwaan R. (Eds) Naturalistic text comprehension. Ablex Publishing, Norwood, NJ, pp. 1–8

    Google Scholar 

  • Vergara, M. (1999). Diseño y aplicación de encuestas de intereses en temas para lectura y escritura [Design and administration of reading and writing tests of interests]. In: G. Parodi (Ed.), Conexiones entre comprensión y producción de textos escritos en alumnos de educación básica [Reading-writing connections in elementary school students] (pp. 47–71). Valparaíso: Pontificia Universidad Católica de Valparaíso

  • Weisser C., Dobrin S. (2001) Ecocomposition: Theoretical and pedagogical approaches. State University of New York Press, Albany, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan R., Radvansky G. (1998) Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin 123: 162–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

This work was supported by FONDECYT Research Grant No. 1980/311.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Giovanni Parodi.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Writing task

Taller de Escritura 1

Nombre:...........................................................................................................

Colegio:...........................................................................................................

Curso: 8° año básico                    Sexo: ..............................................

   En la actividad que viene a continuación, te invitamos a escribir un texto en el que te pedimos hacer tu mejor esfuerzo. Desde ya, agradecemos tu cooperación.

   Actividad

      Los directores de los canales de televisión han decidido que:

      “Se eliminarán todos los programas acerca de deportes peligrosos”.

Tú has sido elegido para comentar acerca de esta decisión en la nueva Revista del Colegio.

Esta revista será enviada a los directores de los canales de televisión.

      Usa estas dos hojas para redactar tu artículo a ser publicado muy pronto.

      Tiempo asignado: 45 minutos aproximadamente.

Appendix 1: Writing task (translation)

Writing Workshop 1

Name:...........................................................................................................

School:...........................................................................................................

Level: 8th grade

   In this activity, we invite you to write a text in which you are asked to do your best effort.

   We appreciate your cooperation.

   Activity

      TV channels directors have decided that:

      “All dangerous TV sport programs will be not be included any more”.

You have been chosen to comment on this statement and write and essay for the new school journal.

This issue will be sent to TV channels directors.

      You can use these two sheets of paper to write your text.

      You are given 45 minutes for this task.

Appendix 2: Reading test and inferential questions

Sabías Tú Que...

  1. (1)

    Desde la antigüedad, al hombre le ha gustado contemplar la belleza del mundo que lo rodea y gozar con los colores, formas y movimientos de todos los elementos de la naturaleza. Su mente inquieta lo ha llevado no sólo a observar el mundo, sino también a estudiarlo científicamente a través de diversos medios a los que puede recurrir. Sin duda, entre los múltiples avances científicos, los instrumentos que registran imágenes son algunos de los inventos que más han contribuido al desarrollo de la ciencia.

  2. (2)

    Pocas personas han pensado en la importancia de los instrumentos que registran imágenes. Sin embargo, resulta interesante pensar en los beneficios que trae consigo esta actividad. Durante años, las personas morían de lo que, al parecer, era un dolor de estómago. Actualmente, muchos enfermos pueden mejorar gracias a que la ecotomografía, permite examinar el estómago del paciente. También es posible detectar tumores cerebrales con aparatos tan valiosos como el escáner. Además, hoy es posible controlar las etapas del embarazo y comprobar el crecimiento del feto con instrumentos especiales. Se puede decir que los instrumentos para registrar imágenes han contribuido a detectar a tiempo problemas relacionados con la salud.

  3. (3)

    Para estudiar el interior del cuerpo humano, los científicos inventaron la ecografía, lo que resultó ser un aporte esencial para los ginecólogos. Estos últimos están interesados en el crecimiento del feto. Este procedimiento entrega datos precisos del desarrollo del ser que está por nacer. E incluso, durante el embarazo, permite obtener información acerca de la maduración del aparato respiratorio, que resulta fundamental para que el cuerpo humano funcione. La inmadurez del sistema de respiración impide que el recién nacido pueda sobrevivir. Durante mucho tiempo no se supo cómo resolver este problema. Hoy en día, los médicos mantienen al bebé en incubadoras por el tiempo que sea necesario.

  4. (4)

    Por otra parte, se puede mencionar otra contribución de los instrumentos que captan imágenes. Por medio de ellos se ha podido registrar el movimiento de los astros en el espacio, a menos que las condiciones atmosféricas no sean las mejores. Como ocurrió, por ejemplo, con el eclipse del año 1998. Los astrónomos se concentraron en el norte de Chile para observar mejor este fenómeno, debido a la claridad de su cielo.

  5. (5)

    Los científicos dedican gran parte de su tiempo a describir el universo. G. Dupont ha realizado uno de los avances más interesantes en el área. Este científico demostró que se podía fotografiar una sola imagen del universo con la ubicación de miles de estrellas. Debido a este descubrimiento, los investigadores franceses han podido observar algunos astros tal como se ordenan en el universo. Esta tarea es una de las metas que se ha planteado la astronomía. Sin el uso de la fotografía, se habrían demorado meses en ubicar estas estrellas en un solo mapa.

  6. (6)

    Sin importar las técnicas empleadas, es indudable que los instrumentos que registran imágenes han permitido el avance científico en diversas áreas. Se puede esperar que en un futuro cercano, el hombre sea capaz de inventar instrumentos con un nivel tecnológico cada vez más especializados. Estos avances permitirán a los investigadores descubrir cosas insospechadas hasta ahora.

Open questions:

  1. 1.

    De acuerdo con el texto, ¿qué procedimiento entrega datos precisos del desarrollo del ser que está por nacer?

  2. 2.

    ¿Cuál es la idea principal del párrafo 4?

  3. 3.

    Según el texto, ¿qué desea probar el autor?

  4. 4.

    Señala las razones más importantes que da el autor para probar la respuesta anterior.

  5. 5.

    De acuerdo con el texto, ¿por qué los médicos mantienen al recién nacido en incubadoras?

  6. 6.

    ¿Cuál es la idea principal del párrafo 2?

  7. 7.

    De acuerdo con el texto, ¿a qué conclusión llega el autor?

  8. 8.

    Según el texto, ¿quiénes se habrían demorado meses en ubicar todas las estrellas en un mismo mapa?

  9. 9.

    Escribe un resumen del texto en tres líneas.

Appendix 2: Reading test and inferential questions (translation)

Do you know that...?

  1. (1)

    From old times, man liked to look at the beauty of the world that surrounds him and enjoy the colors, forms and movement of the nature elements. His wondering mind has taken him not only to observe the world but also to study it scientifically through different devices. Of course, devices that help capture images are some of the most incredible scientific advances and they have contributed much to science development.

  2. (2)

    Few people have thought about the importance of the devices to register images. However, it is interesting to consider the benefits that they bring with them. During years, people died due to what thought stomach pain. Nowadays, many ill people can get better thanks to echotomographies that allow doctors to examine the patient’s stomach. Also, it is possible to detect brain tumors with devices such as scanners. Besides, it is possible to control the baby growing in the mother’s womb and to exam the baby’s healthy growing up. It is possible to say that these special devices to capture images have contributed immensely to detect on time health problems.

  3. (3)

    To study the inner part of the human body, scientists invented the echography, which turned out to be a tremendous help for doctors. They are very much interested in the healthy growing of the fetus. This procedure gives the doctor precise information of the baby. During pregnancy, it helps get information about the maturity of the baby’s breathing system, which is fundamental to keep the human body working properly. The breathing system immaturity is a major cause of death among babies. During a long time, there were no answer to solve this problem; nowadays, doctors keep immature babies in special devices until they are big enough to survive.

  4. (4)

    On the other hand, it is also possible to mention another contribution of the devices that captures images. Using them properly, far away stars have been studied in detail, given the adequate weather conditions. For example, during the year 1998 scientists gathered together in the South of Chile to observe an eclipse, due to the special and clear sky of this region.

  5. (5)

    Scientists spend a lot of time describing the universe. G. Dupont has produced one of the most interesting advances in this area of research. This scientist demonstrated that he could get with only one photograph of the universe the position of a lot of stars. Due to these findings, French researchers have been able to observe some stars in the right order in the universe. This step is only one of the many tasks that astronomy is interested in. Without the use of photographs, it would have taken lots of months to find the right place of some stars in just one map of the universe.

  6. (6)

    No matter what kind of technique could be used to get images, it is undoubtedly true that without these devices the scientific advance would have not been the same in many research areas. It can be expected that in a near future man will be able to invent technological devices each time more specialized. These advances will help researchers discover unexpected unthinkable things.

Open questions:

  1. 1.

    According to the text, which procedure does give precise data about the evolution of the baby?

  2. 2.

    Which is the main idea of paragraph 4?

  3. 3.

    According to the text, which is the author’s purpose?

  4. 4.

    Explain the most important reasons given by the author to prove his thesis.

  5. 5.

    Why do doctors keep babies in special machines after having been delivered?

  6. 6.

    Which is the main idea of paragraph 2?

  7. 7.

    According to the text, which is the author’s conclusion?

  8. 8.

    Based on your reading, who would have taken months in searching stars in the same map?

  9. 9.

    Write a summary of the text in three lines.

Appendix 3: Scoring guideline for argumentative discourse (reading and writing)

Level

Scoring

Total

Microstructural

   Nominal co-reference

1

3

5

5

   Nominal ellipsis

1

3

5

5

   Cause–effect relation

1

3

5

5

Macrostructural

   Macroproposition 1

1

5

9

9

   Macroproposition 2

1

5

9

9

   Topic

1

5

9

9

Superstructural

   Thesis

1

5

9

9

   Arguments

1

5

9

9

   Conclusion

1

5

9

9

Total Test

69

  1. Explanation: The scoring numbers were organized according to the weight we wanted to give to each psycholinguistic skill involved. Therefore, local coherence was given a maximum of 5 points for each feature (total 15). Macrostructural and superstructural levels, because of their higher importance in discourse processing, were given a maximum of 9 points (total 27). The numbers 3 or 5 in the medium scale were given when the expected process was not wrong but the answer was incomplete or the text produced was partly cohesive or coherent at the required level.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Parodi, G. Reading–writing connections: Discourse-oriented research. Read Writ 20, 225–250 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9029-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9029-7

Keywords

Navigation