Skip to main content
Log in

Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: an examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study 149 kindergarten children were assessed for knowledge of letter names and letter sounds, phonological awareness, and cognitive abilities. Through this it examined child and letter characteristics influencing the acquisition of alphabetic knowledge in a naturalistic context, the relationship between letter-sound knowledge and letter-name knowledge, and the prediction of Grade 1 phonological awareness and word identification from these variables. Knowledge of letter sounds was better for vowels and for letters with consonant–vowel names than for those with vowel–consonant names or names bearing little relationship to their sounds. However, there were anomalies within each category reflecting characteristics of the individual letters. Structural equation modelling showed that cognitive ability, comprising receptive vocabulary, non-verbal reasoning, rapid automatized naming of colours, and phonological memory significantly contributed to alphabetic knowledge and phonological awareness. In turn, letter-name knowledge but not phonological awareness predicted letter-sound knowledge and subsequent reading skill.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M., Treiman, R., & Pressley, M. (1998). Reading, writing, and literacy. In I.␣E.␣Siegel & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Child psychology in practice (Vol. 4, pp. 275–355). New York: Wiley

  • Arbuckle J. L., Wothke W. (1999). Amos 4.0 user’s guide. Smallwaters, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron R. B. (1994). The sound-to-spelling connection: Orthographic activation in auditory word recognition and its implication for the acquisition of phonological awareness and literacy skills. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrect, Netherlands pp 219–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaiklock K. E. (2004). The importance of letter knowledge in the relationship between phonological awareness and reading Journal of Research in Reading, 27: 36–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers P. G., Swanson L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51:195–219

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey J. A. (1994). Phonological sensitivity in novice readers and non-readers Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58:134–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey J. A. (1995). Socioeconomic status differences in preschool phonological sensitivity and first-grade reading achievement Journal of Educational Psychology 87: 476–487

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley L., Bryant P. E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne M. W., Cudeck R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. Bollen, J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models. Sage, Newbury Park, CA, pp 136–162

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgess S. R., Lonigan C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70: 117–141

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus A. G., van Ijzendoorn M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies Journal of Educational Psychology, 91: 403–414

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne B., Fielding-Barnsley R. (1989). Acquiring the alphabetic principle: A case for teaching recognition of phoneme identity Journal of Educational Psychology, 82: 805–812

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calfee, R. C., & Drum, P. A. (1979). How the researcher can help the reading teacher with classroom assessment. In L. B. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 2, pp. 173–205). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum

  • Castles A., Coltheart M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91: 77–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chall J. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. McGraw Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaney C. (1992). Language development, metalinguistic awareness, and emergent literacy skills of 3-year-old children in relation to social class Applied Psycholinguistics 15: 371–394

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronin B., Carver P. (1998). Phonological sensitivity, rapid naming, and beginning reading Applied Psycholinguistics 19: 447–461

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Abreu M., Cardoso-Martins C. (1998). Alphabetic access route between beginning reading acquisition in Portuguese: The role of letter-name knowledge Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 10: 85–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong P. F., van der Leij A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study Journal of Educational Psychology, 91: 450–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson D. K., Snow C. E. (1987). Interrelationships among prereading and oral language skills in kindergarteners from two social classes Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2: 1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn L. M., Dunn L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. American Guidance Services, Circle Pines, Minnesota

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri L. C. (1983). A critique of five studies related to letter-name knowledge and learning to read. In L. M. Gentile, M. L. Kamil, J. S. Blanchard (Eds.), Reading research revisited. Merrill, Columbus, OH pp. 143–153

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri L. Deffner N. D., Wilce L. S. (1984). Pictorial mnemonics for phonics Journal of Educational Psychology, 76: 880–893

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher J. M., Satz P. (1979). Unitary deficit hypotheses of reading disabilities Journal of Reading Disabilities, 12: 22–26

    Google Scholar 

  • Gesell A., Amatruda C. S. (1947). Developmental diagnosis (2). Paul B. Hoeber, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., & Gallimore R. (1995). Immigrant Latino parents’ values and beliefs about their children’s education: Continuities and discontinuities across cultures and generations. In P. R. Pintrich & M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: Culture, ethnicity, and motivation (Vol. 9, pp. 183–228). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press

  • Hoyle R. H., Panter A. T. (1995). Writing about structural equation models. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modelling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Sage, Thousand Oaks, pp. 158–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Jansky J. J., de Hirsch K. (1972). Preventing reading failure. Harper & Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline R. B. (1998). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling. Guilford, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin I., Aram D. (2004). Children’s letter names contribute to early literacy. In D. Ravid, H. Bat-Zeev Shyldkrot (Eds.), Perspectives on language and language development. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp. 223–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Lomax R. G., McGee L. M. (1987). Young children’s concepts about print and reading: Toward a model of word acquisition Reading Research Quarterly, 22: 237–256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan C., Burgess S. R., Anthony J. L., Barker T. A. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in two to five year-old children Journal of Educational Psychology, 90: 294–311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason J. M. (1980). When do children begin to learn to read: An exploration of four-year-old children’s letter and word reading competencies Reading Research Quarterly, 15: 203–227

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayzner M. S., Tresselt M. E. (1965). Tables of single-letter and digram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combination Psychonomic Monograph Supplement, 1: 13–32

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87: 179–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang C. (1999). The ABC’s of the ABC’s: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45: 285–308

    Google Scholar 

  • McCormik C., Mason J. M. (1981). What happens to kindergarten children’s knowledge about reading after a summer vacation? The Reading Teacher, 35: 164–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and non-word repetition as a function of vocabulary development Journal of Educational Psychology, 91: 3–19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read. National Institute of Child Health and Development, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus G. F., Swank P. R. (2002). Understanding the relations between RAN letter subtest components and word reading in first-grade students Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35: 158–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parilla R., Kirby J. R., McQuarrie L. (2004). Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development Scientific Studies of Reading, 8: 3–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington B. F., Lefly D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at risk for dyslexia Child Development, 72: 816–833

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reid D., Hresko W., Hammill D. (1989). Test of Early Reading Ability-2. Pro-Ed, Austin, TX

    Google Scholar 

  • Richgels D. J. (1986). An investigation of preschool and kindergarten children’s spelling and reading abilities Journal of Research and Development in Education, 19: 41–47

    Google Scholar 

  • Sattler J. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). Jerome M. Sattler, San Diego, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough H. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability. York Press, Timonium, MD, pp. 75–119

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider C., Fletcher K. M., Francis D. J., Carlson C. D., Foorman B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis Journal of Educational Psychology, 96: 265–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share D. L. (2004). Knowing letter names and learning letter sounds: A causal connection Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88: 213–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith C. L., Tager Flushberg H. (1982). Metalinguistic awareness and language development Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34: 449–468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich K. E. (1985). Explaining the variability in reading ability in terms of psychological processes: What have we learned? Annals of Dyslexia, 35: 67–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 307–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Canada (1996). Census. Retrieved May 10, 2003. www.statscan.ca

  • Stevenson H. W., Neuman R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading Child Development, 57: 646–659

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart M., Colheart M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30: 139–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson H. L., Trainin G., Necoechea D. M., Hammill D. D. (2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading: A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence Review of Educational Research, 73: 407–444

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike E. L., Hagen E. P., Sattler J. M., (1986). Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale- Fourth Edition. Riverside Publishing, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen J. K., Bryant W. (1994). Test of Phonological Awareness. Pro-ed, Austin, TX

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman R. (1994). Use of consonant letter names in beginning spelling Developmental Psychology, 30: 567–580

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In. H. W. Reese, & R. Kail (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol.␣31, pp. 105–135). San Diego: Academic Press

  • Treiman R., Kessler B., Bourassa B. (2001). Children’s own names influence their spelling Applied Psycholinguistics, 22: 555–570

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman R., Tincoff R., Rodriguez K., Mouzaki A., Francis D. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters Child Development, 69: 1524–1540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman R., Zukowski A. (1996). Children’s sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes and phonemes Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61: 193–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer W. E., Herriman M. L., Nesdale A. R. (1988). Metalinguistic abilities and beginning reading Reading Research Quarterly, 23: 134–158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos K. P., Zijlstra B. J. H., lutje Spelberg H. C. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colors, and pictured objects, and word-reading speed Scientific Studies of Reading, 61: 25–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino F. R., Fletcher J. M., Snowling M. J., Scanlon D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45: 2–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner R., Torgesen J. K., Rashotte C. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study Developmental Psychology, 30: 73–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh D. J., Price G. G., Gillingham M. G. (1988). The critical but transitory importance of letter naming Reading Research Quarterly, 23: 108–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler D. A. (1989). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale for Children-Revised. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows D. M. (1993). Visual processes in learning disabilities. In B. Y. L. Wong (Ed.), Learning about learning disabilities. Academic Press, New York, pp. 163–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences Reading Research Quarterly, 26: 123–141

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M. (1997). A provisional, integrative account of phonological and naming-speed deficits in dyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. In B. A. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition: Implications for early intervention. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 67–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M., Bally H., Morris R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers Child Development, 57: 988–1000

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M., Bowers P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias Journal of Educational Psychology, 91: 415–438

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock R. W. (1987). The Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised. American Guidance, Circle Pines, MN

    Google Scholar 

  • Worden P. E., Boettcher W. (1990). Young children’s acquisition of alphabetic knowledge Journal of Reading Behavior, 22: 277–295

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

This research was supported by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to the first author. Thank you is extended to the participating schools and children and to Ian Newby-Clark for his orientation to AMOS. Michelle Bell, Shelly Moretti and Jodi Page have since graduated from the University of Guelph

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mary Ann Evans.

Additional information

This research was supported by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to the first author. Thank you is extended to the participating schools and children and to Ian Newby-Clark for his orientation to AMOS. Michelle Bell, Shelly Moretti and Jodi Page have since graduated from the University of Guelph

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Evans, M.A., Bell, M., Shaw, D. et al. Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: an examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Read Writ 19, 959–989 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9026-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9026-x

Keywords

Navigation