Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Psychosocial Correlates of Interpersonal Sensitivity: A Meta-Analysis

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This meta-analysis examines how interpersonal sensitivity (IS), defined as accurate judgment or recall of others’ behavior or appearance, is related to psychosocial characteristics of the perceiver, defined as personality traits, social and emotional functioning, life experiences, values, attitudes, and self-concept. For 215 independent studies reported in 96 published sources, higher IS was generally associated with favorable or adaptive psychosocial functioning. Significant mean correlations were found for 27 of the 40 categories of psychosocial variables; these categories covered many different personality traits, indicators of mental health, and social and work-related competencies. Moreover, many additional studies that fell outside these conceptual categories also showed significant positive relations between IS and numerous other psychosocial variables. Taken together, the results support the construct validity of IS tests and demonstrate that IS is associated with many important aspects of personal and social functioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. No predictions were made regarding moderators and, due to the typically small number of studies within a given analysis, different potential moderators were very unevenly distributed as well as highly confounded with each other. Although it would have been interesting to compare effects for different cue channels, different test contents (e.g., judgments of emotion versus judgments of personality, or between different specific emotions), it was not feasible to do so. Therefore, not much formal testing of moderators was done.

  2. In a few studies, test-specific accuracy of self-assessments was also calculated in a different way, as the average within-participant correlation between the participant’s assessment of accuracy made after each test item and actual accuracy across those same items (Patterson et al. 2001; Smith et al. 1991). For this definition of self-accuracy, the results were slightly stronger than those shown in the first line of Table 4.

  3. Though the conceptual similarities between the Davis and Kraus (1997) review and the present one are considerable, there are also differences in (1) the number of studies included, (2) the inclusion/exclusion criteria for what was included as a potential psychosocial correlate of IS, (3) the inclusion/exclusion criteria for what kinds of IS tests were included, (4) the inclusion/exclusion criteria for sample characteristics, and (5) how the psychosocial variables were categorized. Regarding the latter, considering that the category schemes for the psychosocial variables were inductively derived in both reviews, it is not surprising that some would be cross-cutting, labeled differently, etc. It is more important to acknowledge that the two reviews often reached the same conclusion than to discuss in detail all of the methodological differences.

References

Note: Entries marked with “D” (for data) contributed quantitative data to the meta-analysis; entries marked with “P” (for psychosocial) are the sources for the published psychosocial variables; and entries marked with “T” (for test) are the citations for the published interpersonal sensitivity tests listed in Table 2.

  • Aamodt, M. G., & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar? A meta-analysis of individual differences in detecting deception. Forensic Examiner, 15, 6–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allik, J., Laidra, K., Realo, A., & Pullmann, H. (2002). Personality development from 12 to 18 years of age: Changes in mean levels and structure of traits. European Journal of Personality, 18, 445–462. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W., & Kramer, B. M. (1946). Some roots of prejudice. Journal of Psychology, 22, 9–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W., Vernon, P. E., & Lindzey, G. (1960). A study of values (3rd ed.). Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Aloni, M., & Bernieri, F. J. (2004). Is love blind? The effects of experience and infatuation on the perception of love. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 287–295. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., & Richeson, J. A. (2000). Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 201–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., & Gray, H. M. (2002). On being sad and mistaken: Mood effects on the accuracy of thin-slice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 947–961. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N., Hallahan, M., & Rosenthal, R. (1995). On judging and being judged accurately in zero-acquaintance situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 518–529. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, D. R., & Kammrath, L. K. (2004). Mind-reading and metacognition: Narcissism, not actual competence, predicts self-estimated ability. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 187–209. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ames, D. R., Rose, P., & Anderson, C. P. (2006). The NPI-16 as a short measure of narcissism. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 440–450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrzejewski, S. A., Hall, J. A., & Salib, E. R. (2009). Anti-Semitism and identification of Jewish group membership from photographs. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 47–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Archer, D., & Akert, R. M. (1977). Words and everything else: Verbal and nonverbal cues in social interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 443–449. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Aube, J., & Whiffen, V. E. (1996). Depressive styles and social acuity: Further evidence for distinct interpersonal correlates of dependency and self-criticism. Communication Research, 23, 407–424. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Babchuk, W. A., Hames, R. B., & Thompson, R. A. (1985). Sex differences in the recognition of infant facial expressions of emotion: The primary caretaker hypothesis. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 89–101. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Barchard, K. A. (2003). Does emotional intelligence assist in the prediction of academic success? Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 840–858. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Social intelligence and decoding of nonverbal cues. Intelligence, 13, 263–287. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Applied Disciplines, 42, 241–251. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001b). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bastone, L. M., & Wood, H. A. (1997). Individual differences in the ability to decode emotional facial expressions. Psychology: A Journal of Human Behavior, 34, 32–36. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Baum, K. M., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (1998). Perception of emotion: Measuring decoding accuracy of adult prosodic cues varying in intensity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 89–107. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, L., & Feldman, R. S. (1989). Enhancing children’s decoding of facial expression. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 269–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Manual for the Revised Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, H., & Prince, J. D. (1994). Alexithymia and the interpretation of emotion-relevant information. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 231–244. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J. (1991). Interpersonal sensitivity in teaching interactions. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 17, 98–103. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J. (2001). Toward a taxonomy of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 3–20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J., & Gillis, J. S. (1995). Personality correlates of accuracy in a social perception task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, 168–170. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, W. M., & Davis, M. H. (1982). Perspective-taking, self-consciousness, and accuracy in person perception. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 3, 1–19. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Blatt, S. J., D’Affliti, J., & Quinlan, D. (1976). Experiences of depression in young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 65, 383–389. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borman, W. C. (1979). Individual differences correlates of accuracy in evaluating others’ performance effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychological Measurement, 3, 103–115. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., Harding, J., & Gallwey, M. (1958). The measurement of skill in social perception. In D. C. McClelland (Ed.), Talent and society: New perspectives in the identification of talent (pp. 29–111). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Buck, R. (1976). A test of nonverbal receiving ability: Preliminary studies. Human Communication Research, 2, 162–171. (T, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1986). The effects of vocalics and nonverbal sensitivity on compliance: A replication and extension. Human Communication Research, 13, 126–144. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K. (1976). The Unwillingness-to-Communicate Scale: Development and validation. Communication Monographs, 43, 60–69. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Busby, D. M., Christensen, C., Crane, D. R., & Larson, J. H. (1995). A revision of the dyadic adjustment scale for use with distressed and nondistressed couples: Construct hierarchy and multidimensional scales. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 21, 289–308. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Byron, K. (2002). Predicting work place success from nonverbal communication skills. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Department of Psychology, Georgia State University. Data reported in Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (D).

  • Byron, K. (2007). Male and female managers’ ability to ‘read’ emotions: Relationships with supervisor’s performance ratings and subordinates’ satisfaction ratings. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80, 713–733.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byron, K., Terranova, S., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2007). Nonverbal emotion recognition and salespersons: Linking ability to perceived and actual success. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 2600–2619.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R. J., Kagan, N., & Krathwohl, D. R. (1971). The development and validation of a scale to measure affective sensitivity (empathy). Journal of Counseling Psychology, 18, 407–412. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Carney, D. R., & Harrigan, J. A. (2003). It takes one to know one: Interpersonal sensitivity is related to accurate assessments of others’ interpersonal sensitivity. Emotion, 3, 194–200. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, J. D., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Individual differences in the accuracy of detecting social covariations: Ecological sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 439–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carton, J. S., Kessler, E. A., & Pape, C. L. (1999). Nonverbal decoding skills and relationship well-being in adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 91–101. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 330–339. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, N. D., Wolcott-Burnam, S., Janovics, J. E., Burns, G. N., & Quirk, S. W. (2005). The good judge revisited: Individual differences in the accuracy of personality judgments. Journal of Human Performance, 18, 123–149. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cline, V. B. (1955). Ability to judge personality assessed with a stress interview and sound-film technique. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 183–187. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, W. W., Leeds, C. H., & Callis, R. (1951). Minnesota Teacher Attitude Inventory. New York: The Psychological Corporation. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, W. W., & Medley, D. M. (1954). Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 414–418. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooley, E. L. (2005). Attachment style and decoding of nonverbal cues. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 25–34. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PR-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, M. (1992). Training students to decode verbal and nonverbal cues: Effects of confidence and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 308–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behavior of others: The Interpersonal Perception Task. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 225–245. (T, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, M., & Philpott, J. (1986). Predictors of therapeutic talent in aspiring clinicians: A multivariate analysis. Psychotherapy, 23, 363–369. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, M. R. (1977). Personality and the structure of the nonverbal communication of emotion. Journal of Personality, 45, 564–584. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–146. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H., & Kraus, L. A. (1997). Personality and empathic accuracy. In W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic accuracy (pp. 144–168). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davitz, J. R., Beldoch, M., Blau, S., Dimitrovsky, L., Levitt, E., Kempner Levy, P., et al. (1964). Personality, perceptual, and cognitive correlates of emotional sensitivity. In J. R. Davitz (Ed.), The communication of emotional meaning (pp. 57–68). New York: McGraw-Hill. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M., Charlton, K., Cooper, H., Lindsay, J. J., & Muhlenbruck, L. (1997). The accuracy-confidence correlation in the detection of deception. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 346–357.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DiMatteo, M. R., Friedman, H. S., & Taranta, A. (1979). Sensitivity to bodily nonverbal communication as a factor in practitioner-patient rapport. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 4, 18–26. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • DiMatteo, M. R., Hays, R. D., & Prince, L. M. (1986). Relationship of physicians’ nonverbal communication skills to patient satisfaction, appointment noncompliance, and physician workload. Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 581–594. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Downey, G., & Feldman, S. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, D. G. (1995). A scale for measuring propensity for abusiveness. Journal of Family Violence, 10, 203–221. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A. L. (1959). Edwards Personal Preference Schedule manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J., Jackson, H. J., & Pattison, P. E. (2002). Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: A methodological review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 789–832.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1974). Nonverbal behavior and psychopathology. In R. J. Friedman & M. Katz (Eds.), The psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 3–31). Washington, DC: Winston & Sons. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Predicting workplace outcomes from the ability to eavesdrop on feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 963–971. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elfenbein, H. A., Foo, M. D., White, J., Tan, H. H., & Aik, V. C. (2007). Reading your counterpart: The benefit of emotion recognition accuracy for effectiveness in negotiation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 205–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. C. (1968). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Firth, E. A., Conger, J. C., Kuhlenschmidt, S., & Dorcey, T. (1986). Social competence and social perceptivity. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 4, 85–100. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, P. L., Lombardo, J. P., & Simon, L. J. (1987). Relation of sex and sex role to adults’ ability to decode infants’ emotions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 65, 595–600. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, H. S., Prince, L., Riggio, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (1980). Understanding and assessing nonverbal expressiveness: The Affective Communication Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 333–351. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C. (1995). On the accuracy of personality judgment: A realistic approach. Psychological Review, 102, 652–670.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Funder, D. C., & Harris, M. J. (1986). On the several facets of personality assessment: The case of social acuity. Journal of Personality, 54, 528–549. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillis, J. S., Bernieri, F. J., & Wooten, E. (1995). The effects of stimulus medium and feedback on the judgment of rapport. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 63, 33–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7–28). Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, N. E., & Feldman, R. S. (1996). Knowledge of American Sign Language and the ability of hearing individuals to decode facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 111–122. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, H. G. (1957). Manual for the California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, H. G. (1987). California Psychological Inventory: Administrator’s guide. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. L. (1965). The Adjective Check List manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1992). Testing a two dimensional model of adult attachment: A latent variable approach. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo. (P).

  • Grinspan, D., Hemphill, A., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2003). Improving the ability of elementary school-age children to identify emotion in facial expression. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 88–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, A. G. (1983). Family expressiveness styles and nonverbal communication skills. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 14–26. (P, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, A. G. (1986). Family socialization of emotional expression and nonverbal communication styles and skills. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 827–836. (P, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1979). Gender, gender roles, and nonverbal communication skills. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication: Individual differences (pp. 32–67). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (2001). The PONS test and the psychometric approach to measuring interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 143–160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Bernieri, F. J. (2001). Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Bernieri, F. J., & Carney, D. R. (2005). Nonverbal behavior and interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Harrigan, R. Rosenthal, & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), The new handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research (pp. 237–281). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Carter, J. D. (1999). Gender-stereotype accuracy as an individual difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 350–359. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Carter, J. D., & Horgan, T. G. (2001). Status roles and recall of nonverbal cues. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 79–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. W., Gaul, L., & Kent, M. (1999). College students’ perception of facial expressions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 763–770. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1981). Sex roles and nonverbal communication skills. Sex Roles, 7, 273–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Halberstadt, A. G. (1994). “Subordination” and sensitivity to nonverbal cues: A study of married working women. Sex Roles, 31, 149–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Halberstadt, A. G., & O’Brien, C. E. (1997). “Subordination” and nonverbal sensitivity: A study and synthesis of findings based on trait measures. Sex Roles, 37, 295–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., Murphy, N. A., & Schmid Mast, M. (2006). Recall of nonverbal cues: Exploring a new definition of interpersonal sensitivity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 141–155. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Harkness, K. L., Sabbagh, M. A., Jacobson, J. A., Chowdrey, N. K., & Chen, T. (2005). Enhanced accuracy of mental state decoding in dysphoric college students. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 999–1025. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrick, S. S., Dicke, A., & Hendrick, C. (1998). The Relationship Assessment Scale. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 137–142. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, C. E., Siegelman, L., Gronsky, B. R., Sturniolo, F., & Fretz, B. R. (1981). Nonverbal communication and counseling outcome. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, 203–212. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Belch, C. (2000). Interparental violence and nonverbal abilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 3–24. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Koestner, R. (1993). The origins of nonverbal sensitivity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 466–473. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Zuckerman, M. (1990). The effect of nonverbal sensitivity on social interaction. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 14, 155–170. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, R. (1969). Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 307–316. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Buysse, A., Pham, H., Rivers, K., Erickson, J. R., Hancock, M., et al. (2000). On the difficulty of distinguishing “good” and “poor” perceivers: A social relations analysis of empathic accuracy data. Personal Relationships, 7, 219–234. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Stinson, L., Bissonnette, V., & Garcia, S. (1990). Naturalistic social cognition: Empathic accuracy in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 730–742.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iizuka, Y., Patterson, M. L., & Matchen, J. C. (2002). Accuracy and confidence on the Interpersonal Perception Task: A Japanese-American comparison. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 26, 159–174. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1972). Patterns of emotions: A new analysis of anxiety and depression. New York: Academic Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, D. N. (1974). Personality Research Form manual. Goshen, NY: Research Psychologists Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, D. N. (1976). Jackson Personality Inventory manual. Goshen, NY: Research Psychologists Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Janis, I., & Field, P. (1959). Sex differences and personality factors related to persuasibility. In C. Hovland & I. Janis (Eds.), Personality and persuasibility (pp. 55–68). New Haven: Yale University Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenness, A. (1932). The effects of coaching subjects in the recognition of facial expression. Journal of General Psychology, 7, 163–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P. (1989). The BFI-54. Unpublished test, Institute of Personality and Social Research, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley. (P).

  • John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. (1991). The Big-Five Inventory. Technical Report, Institute for Personality and Social Research, University of California, Berkeley, CA. (P).

  • Kanner, L. (1931). Judging emotions from facial expressions. Psychological Monographs, 41, Whole No. 186.

  • Kleinman, B. (2003). The association between the ability to read emotion in nonverbal cues and relationship success. Unpublished honors thesis, Emory University, Atlanta, GA. Data reported in Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (D).

  • Larkin, K. T., Martin, R. R., & McClain, S. E. (2002). Cynical hostility and the accuracy of decoding facial expressions of emotions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 285–292. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., & Schreindorfer, L. S. (2001). Individual differences in the need to belong. Unpublished manuscript, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC. (P).

  • Lee, D. Y., & Hallberg, E. T. (1982). Nonverbal behaviors of “good” and “poor” counselors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29, 414–417. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. Y., Hallberg, E. T., Kocsis, M., & Haase, R. F. (1980). Decoding skills in nonverbal communication and perceived interviewer effectiveness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 27, 89–92. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lembke, A., & Ketter, T. A. (2002). Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 302–304.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lennox, R. D., & Wolfe, R. N. (1984). Revision of the self-monitoring scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1349–1364. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, D., Marziali, E., & Hood, J. (1997). Emotion processing in borderline personality disorders. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185, 240–246.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R. A., & Dietz, J. K. (2000). The relation of gender, personality, and intelligence to judges’ accuracy in judging strangers’ personality from brief video segments. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 25–43. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingston, S. A. (1981). Nonverbal communication tests as predictors of success in psychology and counseling. Applied Psychological Measurement, 5, 325–331. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, H., & Wallace, K. (1959). Short marital adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251–255. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 6–10. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Marangoni, C., Garcia, S., Ickes, W., & Teng, G. (1995). Empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant setting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 854–869. (T, D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, A. A., Kozak, M. N., & Ambady, N. (2007). Accurate identification of fear facial expressions predicts prosocial behavior. Emotion, 7, 239–251.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Ekman, P. (1988). Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE) [slides]. San Francisco: Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., LeRoux, J., Wilson-Cohn, C., Raroque, J., Kooken, K., Ekman, P., et al. (2000). A new test to measure emotion recognition ability: Matsumoto and Ekman’s Japanese and Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test (JACBART). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 179–209. (T, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (1999). Instruction manual for the MSCEIT Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, Research Version 1.1. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion, 3(9), 7–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, E. B., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2001). Associations between social anxiety and nonverbal processing skill in preadolescent boys and girls. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 3–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCroskey, J. C. (1978). Validity of the PRCA as an index of oral communication apprehension. Communication Monographs, 45, 192–203. (P).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntire, K. A., Danforth, M. M., & Schneider, H. G. (1999). Measuring cue perception: Assessment of reliability and validity. North American Journal of Psychology, 1, 261–266. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1970). The development and validation of measures of affiliative tendency and sensitivity to rejection. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 30, 417–428. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal communication. New York: Aldine-Atherton. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40, 525–543. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Ksionsky, S. (1974). A theory of affiliation. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Miczo, N., Segrin, C., & Allspach, L. E. (2001). Relationship between nonverbal sensitivity, encoding, and relational satisfaction. Communication Reports, 14, 39–48. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. (1984). High and low self-monitoring individuals: Their decoding skills and empathic expression. Journal of Personality, 52, 372–388. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Molinsky, A. L., Krabbenhoft, M. A., Ambady, N., & Choi, Y. S. (2005). Cracking the nonverbal code: Intercultural competence and gesture recognition across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 380–395. (D, T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Morand, D. A. (2001). The emotional intelligence of managers: Assessing the construct validity of a nonverbal measure of “people skills”. Journal of Business and Psychology, 16, 21–33. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mumford, D. B. (1998). The measurement of culture shock. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 149–154. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, I. B. (1962). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Princeton: Educational Testing Service. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, L. E. (1977). Counselor characteristics and training as related to the process of empathy and its manifestations. Dissertation Abstracts International, 27, 4138A–4139A. (University Microfilms No. 77-114). (T).

  • Nideffer, R. M. (1976). Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 394–405. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Noller, P. (1981). Gender and marital adjustment level differences in decoding messages from spouses and strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 272–278. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr. (1995). A study of the DANVA-AV in college students. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, Emory University. Data reported in Baum, K. M., & Nowicki, S. Jr. (1998). Perception of emotion: Measuring decoding accuracy of adult prosodic cues varying in intensity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 89–107. (D).

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Duke, M. P. (1974). A locus of control scale for college as well as non-college adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 38, 136–137. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–34. (T).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Hartigan, M. (2001). Accuracy of facial affect recognition as a function of locus of control orientation and anticipated interpersonal interaction. Journal of Social Psychology, 128, 363–372. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Mitchell, J. (1998). Accuracy in identifying affect in child and adult faces and voices and social competence in preschool children. Genetic, Social, and Psychological Monographs, 124, 39–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Richman, D. (1985). The effect of standard, motivation, and strategy instructions on the facial processing accuracy of internal and external subjects. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 354–364. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401–421.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L., Foster, J. L., & Bellmer, C. D. (2001). Another look at accuracy and confidence in social judgments. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 207–219. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: The need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095–1107. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 posture test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (T, D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R., & Kellerman, H. (1974). Emotions Profile Index manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressive. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 160–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Realo, A., Allik, J., Nõlvak, A., Valk, R., Ruus, T., Schmidt, M., et al. (2003). Mind-reading ability: Beliefs and performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 420–445. (P, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., Jr., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred yeas of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E. (1989). Social Skills Inventory manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E., & Carney, D. R. (2003). Social Skills Inventory manual (2nd ed.). Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden, Inc. (P, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E., & Riggio, H. R. (2001). Self-report measurement of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 127–142). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riggio, R. E., Tucker, J., & Coffaro, D. (1989). Social skills and empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 93–99. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Robbins, J. M., Kirmayer, L. J., Cathébras, P., Yaffe, M. J., & Dworkind, M. (1994). Physician characteristics and the recognition of depression and anxiety in primary care. Medical Care, 32, 795–812. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, R. W., Hendin, H. M., & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global self-esteem: Construct validation of a single-item measure and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 151–161. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rokeach, M. (1956). Political and religious dogmatism: An alternative to the authoritarian personality. Psychological Monographs, 70, 425. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton: Princeton University Press. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (revised ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press. (T, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purpose display of magnitude of experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 166–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosip, J. C., & Hall, J. A. (2004). Knowledge of nonverbal cues, gender, and nonverbal decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 267–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, A. D., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A measure of the ability to identify emotion in children’s tone of voice. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 67–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, No. 1 (Whole No. 609). (P).

  • Rotter, J. B. (1967). A new scale for the measurement of interpersonal trust. Journal of Personality, 35, 651–665. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, Z. (1973). Liking and loving: An invitation to social psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, R. S., Munz, D. C., & Bommer, W. H. (2005). Leading from within: The effects of emotion recognition and personality on transformational leadership behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 845–858. (D).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, D. W., & Cutrona, C. E. (1988). Development and evolution of the UCLA Loneliness Scale. Unpublished manuscript, Center for Health Services Research, College of Medicine, University of Iowa, Ames. (P).

  • Russell, R. L., Stokes, J., Jones, M. E., Czogalik, D., & Rohleder, L. (1993). The role of nonverbal sensitivity in childhood psychopathology. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 69–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryder, R. G. (1973). Longitudinal data relating marriage satisfaction to having a child. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 35, 604–606. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 1069–1081. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabatelli, R. M., Buck, R., & Dreyer, A. (1980). Communication via facial cues in intimate dyads. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 242–247. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabatelli, R. M., Buck, R., & Dreyer, A. (1983). Locus of control, interpersonal trust, and nonverbal communication accuracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 399–409. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabetelli, R. M., Buck, R., & Dreyer, A. (1982). Nonverbal communication accuracy in married couples: Relationship with marital complaints. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1088–1097. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schag, D., Loo, C., & Levin, M. M. (1978). The Group Assessment of Interpersonal Traits (GAIT): Differentiation of measures and their relationship to behavioral response modes. American Journal of Community Psychology, 6, 47–62. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, J., & Schneider-Duker, M. (1984). Sex roles and nonverbal sensitivity. Journal of Social Psychology, 122, 281–282. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, J. E. (1995). Self-concept, social anxiety, and interpersonal skills. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 955–958. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, J. E., & Ketrow, S. M. (1997). Social anxiety and performance in an interpersonal perception task. Psychological Reports, 81, 991–996. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schweinle, W. E., Ickes, W., & Bernstein, I. H. (2002). Empathic inaccuracy in husband to wife aggression: The overattribution bias. Personal Relationships, 9, 141–158. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shah, N. (2001). The association between nonverbal communication skill and depression and anxiety. Unpublished master’s thesis, Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA. Data reported in Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shin, H., & Abell, N. (1999). The Homesickness and Contentment scale: Developing a culturally sensitive measure of adjustment for Asians. Research on Social Work Practice, 9, 45–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, L. J., Francis, P. L., & Lombardo, J. P. (1990). Sex, sex-role, and Machiavellianism as correlates of decoding ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 243–247. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. J., Archer, D., & Costanzo, M. (1991). “Just a hunch”: Accuracy and awareness in person perception. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 3–18. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537. (P, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, M. (1987). Public appearances, private realities: The psychology of self-monitoring. New York: Freeman. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity & femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates, & antecedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. (1985). Assessment of state and trait anxiety: Conceptual and methodological issues. The Southern Psychologist, 2(4), 6–16. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzberg, B. H. (1995). The Conversation Skills Rating Scale. Annandale, VA: Speech Communication Association. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., & Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 37–55. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The conflict tactics (CT) scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75–88. (P).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stricker, L. J., & Rock, D. A. (1990). Interpersonal competence, social intelligence, and general ability. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 833–839. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, N. D. (1952). The relationship of psychotherapeutic skill and experience to knowledge of other people. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota. (T).

  • Surguladze, S. A., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brébion, G., Travis, M. J., & Phillips, M. L. (2004). Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression. Neuropsychology, 18, 212–218.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swenson, J., & Casmir, F. L. (1998). The impact of culture-sameness, gender, foreign travel, and academic background on the ability to interpret facial expression of emotion in others. Communication Quarterly, 46, 214–230. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Taft, R. (1955). The ability to judge people. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 1–23.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terranova, S. (2002). Predicting success of salespeople. Unpublished master’s thesis, Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA. Data reported in Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (D).

  • Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Husain, G. (2004). Decoding speech prosody: Do music lessons help? Emotion, 4, 46–64. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tickle-Degnen, L. (1998). Working well with others: The prediction of students’ clinical performance. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 52, 133–142. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toner, H. L., & Gates, G. R. (1985). Emotional traits and recognition of facial expression of emotion. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 48–66. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Trimboli, A., & Walker, M. (1993). The CAST test of nonverbal sensitivity. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 12, 49–65. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, P. E. (1933). Some characteristics of the good judge of personality. Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 42–58. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, D. S., & Colvin, R. C. (2003). Interpersonal orientation and the accuracy of personality judgments. Journal of Personality, 71, 267–295. (D).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1991). The Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of Iowa, Department of Psychology, Iowa City. (P).

  • Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of the social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448–457. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, L., Reis, H., & Nezlek, J. (1983). Loneliness, social interaction, and sex roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 943–953. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehorn, J. C., & Betz, B. (1954). A study of psychotherapeutic relationships between physicians and schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 321–331. (P).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickline, V. B. (2002, August). Predicting cultural adjustment outcomes from nonverbal and verbal communication skills. Paper presented for the Psi Chi Research Award at the American Psychological Association conference, Chicago, IL. Data reported in Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (D).

  • Wyner, D. R. (2000). The ability to decode nonverbal cues of emotion: A potential characteristic of NCAA Division III athletes who contribute to team cohesion, and demonstrate skills in coordinating sports play. Unpublished master’s thesis, Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, GA. Data reported in Pitterman, H., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (2004). A test of the ability to identify emotion in human standing and sitting postures: The Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy-2 Posture Test (DANVA2-POS). Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 130, 146–162. (D).

  • Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (no date). The Interpersonal Perception Task-15 (IPT-15): A guide for researchers and teachers. Berkeley, CA: University of California Extension. (T).

  • Yoo, S. H., Matsumoto, J. A., & LeRoux, J. A. (2005). The influence of emotion recognition and emotion regulation on intercultural adjustment. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 345–363. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaidel, S. F., & Mehrabian, A. (1969). The ability to communicate and infer positive and negative attitudes facially and vocally. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 3, 233–241. (D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zebrowitz, L. A. (2001). Groping for the elephant of interpersonal sensitivity. In J. A. Hall & F. J. Bernieri (Eds.), Interpersonal sensitivity: Theory and measurement (pp. 333–350). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., & Larrance, D. T. (1979). Individual differences in perceived encoding and decoding abilities. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.), Skill in nonverbal communication (pp. 171–203). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn, & Hain. (P, D).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M., Larrance, D. T., Hall, J. A., DeFrank, R. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1979). Posed and spontaneous communication of emotion via facial and vocal cues. Journal of Personality, 47, 712–733. (D).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Lisa Babin, Michelle Cowdrick, and Halle Thurnauer for help with retrieval and coding of articles.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judith A. Hall.

Additional information

This article is dedicated to Robert Rosenthal, who inspired the first author and many others to want to study interpersonal sensitivity.

Appendix

Appendix

See Tables 8, 9 and 10.

Table 8 Variables included in positively valenced psychosocial categories
Table 9 Variables included in negatively valenced psychosocial categories
Table 10 Variables included in other psychosocial categories

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hall, J.A., Andrzejewski, S.A. & Yopchick, J.E. Psychosocial Correlates of Interpersonal Sensitivity: A Meta-Analysis. J Nonverbal Behav 33, 149–180 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0070-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-009-0070-5

Keywords

Navigation