Skip to main content
Log in

How do wild baboons (Papio ursinus) plan their routes? Travel among multiple high-quality food sources with inter-group competition

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

How do humans and animals travel between multiple destinations on a given foraging trip? This question is of theoretical and practical interest, yet few empirical data exist to date. We examined how a group of wild chacma baboons travelled among multiple, simultaneously fruiting mountain fig trees (Ficus glumosa). In the course of a 16-month study, this highly preferred fruit was available during a 3-week period, from relatively few sites, which were also utilized by four larger baboon groups. We used directness of route and travel speed of 13 days of observation, and approach rates of 31 days of observation to differentiate between purposeful and opportunistic encounters with 50 fig trees. The study group visited a total of 30 fig trees overall, but only 8 trees per day on average. Each morning, they travelled along a highly repetitive route on all days of observation, thereby visiting 2–4 fig trees. They approached these trees rapidly along highly directed paths without intermittently exploiting other food sources that were available in large quantities. Then, they abruptly changed behaviour, switching to lower travel speed and less directed routes as they foraged on a variety of foods. They approached additional fig trees later in the day, but approach rates were similar to those at times of year when fruit of this fig species was unavailable; this suggested that encounters with trees after the behavioural switch were not planned. Comparing visits to purposefully and opportunistically encountered trees, we found no difference in the average time spent feeding or frequency of feeding supplants, suggesting that purposefully and opportunistically visited trees had similar values. We conclude that when foraging for mountain fig fruit the baboons’ cognitive maps either contain information on relatively few trees or of only a single route along which several trees are situated, leading to very limited planning abilities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Batschelet E (1965) Statistical methods of the analysis of problems in animal orientation and certain biological rhythms. American Institute of Biological Sciences, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleveland WS (1979) Robust locally weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots. J Am Stat Assoc 74:829–836

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cramer A, Gallistel C (1997) Vervet monkeys as travelling salesmen. Nature 387:464

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham E, Janson C (2007) Integrating information about location and value of resources by white-faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia). Anim Cogn 10:293–304

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel C, Cramer A (1996) Computations on metric maps in mammals: getting oriented and choosing a multi-destination route. J Exp Biol 199:211–217

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garber P (1988) Foraging decisions during nectar feeding by tamarin monkeys (Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis, Callitrichidae, Primates) in Amazonian Peru. Biotropica 20:100–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garber P (1989) Role of spatial memory in primate foraging patterns: Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis. Am J Primatol 19:203–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling T, Gärling E (1988) Distance minimization in downtown pedestrian shopping. Environ Plann A 20:547–554

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibeault S, MacDonald SE (2000) Spatial memory and foraging competition in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Primates 41:147–160

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janmaat K, Byrne RW, Zuberbühler K (2006a) Evidence for a spatial memory of fruiting states of rainforest trees in wild mangabeys. Anim Behav 72:797–807

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janmaat K, Byrne RW, Zuberbühler K (2006b) Primates take weather into account when searching for fruits. Curr Biol 16:1232–1237

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janson C (1998) Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 55:1229–1243

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janson C, Byrne RW (2007) What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box. Anim Cog 10:357–367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik A, Bateson M (1996) Risky theories—the effects of variance on foraging decisions. Am Zool 36:402–434

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski J, Fischer J, Call J (2008) Prospective object search in dogs: mixed evidence for knowledge of What and Where. Anim Cogn 11:367–371

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lawler EL, Lenstra JK, Rinnooy AHG, Shmors DB (1990) The traveling salesman problem: a guided tour of combinatorial optimization. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackinnon J (1978) The ape within us. Collins, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Manser M, Bell M (2004) Spatial representation of shelter locations in meerkats, Suricata suricatta. Anim Behav 68:151–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin P, Bateson P (1993) Measuring behaviour. An introductory guide, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Menzel E (1973) Chimpanzee spatial memory organization. Science 182:943–945

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menzel C (1991) Cognitive aspects of foraging in Japanese monkeys. Anim Behav 41:397–402

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menzel C (1997) Primates’ knowledge of their natural habitat: as indicated in foraging. In: Whiten A, Byrne RW (eds) Machiavellian intelligence II: extensions and evaluations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 207–239

    Google Scholar 

  • Milton K (1981) Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as a stimulus to primate mental development. Am Anthropol 83:534–548

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milton K (2000) Quo vadis? Tactics of food search and group movement in primates and other animals. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 375–417

    Google Scholar 

  • Normand E, Boesch C (2009) Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees. Anim Behav 77:1195–1201

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noser R (2004) Ecology, route choice and cognitive maps in wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). PhD Dissertation, School of Psychology. University of St Andrews

  • Noser R, Byrne RW (2007a) Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus. Anim Behav 73:257–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noser R, Byrne RW (2007b) Mental maps in chacma baboons (Papio ursinus): using intergroup encounters as a natural experiment. Anim Cog 10:331–340

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohashia K, Thomsonb JD, D’Souza D (2007) Trapline foraging by bumble bees: IV. Optimization of route geometry in the absence of competition. Behav Ecol 18:1–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohashia K, Leslie A, Thomson JD (2008) Trapline foraging by bumble bees: V. Effects of experience and priority on competitive performance. Behav Ecol 19(5):936–948

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Provenza F (1996) Acquired aversions as the basis for varied diets of ruminants foraging on rangelands. J Anim Sci 74:2010–2020

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigg H, Stolba A (1981) Home range and daily march in a hamadryas baboon troop. Folia Primatol 36:40–75

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tinkelpaugh O (1932) Multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzee and monkeys. J Comp Psychol 13:207–243

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener JM, Tenbrink T (2008) Traveling salesman problem: the human case. Künstliche Intelligenz KI und Kognition 1:18–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener JM, Ehbauer N, Mallot AH (2008) Planning paths to multiple targets: memory involvement and planning heuristics in spatial problem solving. Psych Res. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0181-3

  • Zar J (1999) Biostatistical analysis. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the Parks Board of the Limpopo Province, South Africa, for permission to conduct this study at Blouberg Nature Reserve, Janine and Peter Snyman for logistic support in the field, and Anne Russon and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. This study was supported by grants from Zürcher Hochschulverein, Schweizerische Akademie für Naturwissenschaften, Steo-Stiftung, Goethe-Stiftung, Stiftung Thyll-Dürr, Familien Vontobel-Stiftung, Stiftung Annemarie Schindler, Switzerland, and Russell Trust, Scotland, to R.N.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard W. Byrne.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Noser, R., Byrne, R.W. How do wild baboons (Papio ursinus) plan their routes? Travel among multiple high-quality food sources with inter-group competition. Anim Cogn 13, 145–155 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0254-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0254-8

Keywords

Navigation