Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of nicotine gum on psychomotor performance in smokers and non-smokers

  • Original Investigations
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted to investigate the effects of nicotine on human performance. In the first study six smokers, who had been allowed to smoke normally prior to testing, completed a battery of psychometric tests (choice reaction time, memory scanning, tracking and flicker fusion threshold) at set points over 4 h after chewing 0, 2, or 4 mg nicotine polacrilex gum. A second study followed a similar design, but used five non-smoker volunteers who were required to chew only the 0 or 2 mg nicotine gum. Blood nicotine levels following the gum were measured in all subjects. The results indicate that additional nicotine improved both the speed and accuracy of motor activity among the smokers, but did not enhance central cognitive processes. No drug effects were found in the non-smoker study.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Benowitz NL, Jacob P, Jones RT, Rosenberg J (1982) Interindividual variability in metabolism and cardiovascular effects of nicotine in man. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 221:368–372

    Google Scholar 

  • Benowitz NL, Jacob P, Savanapridi C (1987) Determinants of nicotine intake while chewing nicotine polacrilex gum. Clin Pharmacol Ther 41:467–473

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunne MP, MacDonald D, Hartley LR (1986) The effects of nicotine upon memory and problem solving performance. Physiol Behav 37:849–854

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankenhaeuser M, Myrsten A-L, Post B, Johansson G (1971) Behavioral and physiological effects of cigarette smoking in a monotonous situation. Psychopharmacologia 22:1–7

    Google Scholar 

  • Frewer LJ, Hindmarch I (1988) The effects of time of day, age, and anxiety on a choice reaction task. In: Hindmarch I, Aufdembrinke B, Ott H (eds) Psychopharmacology and reaction time. Wiley, Chichester, pp 103–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakan T (1988) Nicotine induced locomotor activity in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 29:661–663

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley LR (1973) Cigarette smoking and stimulus selection. Br J Psychol 64:593–599

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimstra NW, Bancroft NR, DeKock AR (1967) Effects of smoking upon sustained performance in a simulated driving task. Ann NY Acad Sci 142:295–307

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch I (1980) Psychomotor function and psychoactive drugs. Br J Clin Pharmacol 10:189–209

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch I (1982) Critical flicker fusion frequency (CFFF): The effects of psychotropic compounds. Pharmacopsychiatrica 15:44–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarch I, Subhan Z, Stoker MJ (1983) The effects of zimeldine and amitriptyline on car driving and psychomotor performance. Acta Psychiatr Scand 68:141–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis MJ, Hajek P, Russell MAH, West RJ, Feyerabend C (1987) Nasal nicotine solution as an aid to cigarette withdrawal: a pilot clinical trial. Br J Addict 82:983–988

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan RM, Hatsukami DK, Anton DJ (1989) The effects of short-term smokeless tobacco deprivation on performance. Psychopharmacology 98:126–130

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson PS, Finnegan JK, Haag HB (1950) Observations on the effect of cigarette smoking on the fusion frequency of flicker. J Clin Invest 29:483–485

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangan GL (1982) The effects of cigarette smoking on vigilance performance. J Gen Psychol 106:77–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangan GL, Golding JF (1983) The effects of smoking on memory consolidation. J Psychol 115:65–77

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan SF, Pickens RW (1982) Reaction time performance as a function of cigarette smoking procedure. Psychopharmacology 77:383–386

    Google Scholar 

  • Myrsten A-L, Frankenhaeuser M, Post B, Johansson G (1971) Enhanced behavioral efficiency induced by cigarette smoking as related to different smoking habits. Reports from the Psychological Laboratories, University of Stockholm, No. 378

  • Parrott AC, Winder G (1989) Nicotine chewing gum (2 mg, 4 mg) and cigarette smoking: comparative effects upon vigilance and heart rate. Psychopharmacology 97:251–261

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters R, McGee R (1982) Cigarette smoking and state-dependent memory. Psychopharmacology 76:232–235

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell MAH (1987) Nicotine intake by smokers: are rates of absorption or steady-state levels more important? In: Rand MJ, Thurau K (eds) The pharmacology of nicotine. IRL Press, Oxford, pp 375–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell MAH, Feyerabend C (1978) Cigarette smoking: a dependance on high nicotine boli. Drug Metab Rev 8:29–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell MAH, Feyerabend C, Cole PV (1976) Plasma nicotine levels after cigarette smoking and chewing nicotine gum. Br Med J 1:1043–1046

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder FR, Henningfield JE (1989) Effects of nicotine administration following 12 h of tobacco deprivation: Assessment on computerised performance tasks. Psychopharmacology 97:17–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire LR, Davies NP (1981) The pharmacology of memory: a neurobiological perspective. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 22:323–356

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg S (1966) High speed scanning in human memory. Science 153:652–654

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg S (1975) Memory scanning: new findings and current controversies. Q J Exp Psychol 27:1–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarriere HC, Hartemann F (1964) Investigations into the effects of tobacco smoke on a visual vigilance task. Proceedings of the Second International Congress of Ergonomics, pp 525–530

  • Warburton DM, Wesnes K, Shergold K, James M (1986) Facilitation of learning and state dependancy with nicotine. Psychopharmacology 89:55–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1983) Effects of smoking on rapid visual information processing performance. Neuropsychobiology 9:223–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1984) Effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human rapid information processing performance. Psychopharmacology 82:147–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM, Matz B (1983) Effects of nicotine on stimulus sensitivity and response bias in a visual vigilance task. Neuropsychobiology 9:41–44

    Google Scholar 

  • West RJ, Jarvis MJ (1986) Effects of nicotine on finger tapping rate in non-smokers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 25:727–731

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hindmarch, I., Kerr, J.S. & Sherwood, N. Effects of nicotine gum on psychomotor performance in smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology 100, 535–541 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02244008

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02244008

Key words

Navigation