Skip to main content
Log in

Tolerance of bitter compounds by an herbivore,Cavia porcellus

  • Published:
Journal of Chemical Ecology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many plant defensive chemicals are bitter to humans. Because of this taste characteristic, and because bitter compounds are often toxic, such substances, and the plants that contain them, are regarded as generally unpalatable to wildlife. These assumptions may be unwarranted. To test the hypothesis that herbivores are indifferent to ‘bitter’ tastants, we investigated the responsiveness of guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) to denatonium benzoate, denatonium saccharide, limonene,l-phenylalanine, naringin, quebracho, quinine, Ro-Pel (a commercial animal repellent containing denatonium saccharide) and sucrose octaacetate. Only quinine and sucrose octaacetate slightly but significantly reduced feeding (P<0.05). Our findings are inconsistent with the notion that herbivores generally avoid what humans describe as bitter tastes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Beauchamp, G.K., andMason, J.R. 1991. Comparative hedonics of taste, pp. 159–184,in R.C. Bolles (ed.). The Hedonics of Taste. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belitz, H.D., andWeizer, H. 1985. Bitter compounds: occurrences and structure-activity relationships.Food Rev. Int. 1:271–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, S.F., Cunningham, L.A., Burke, T.J., Richard, M.M., Langley, W.M., andTheis, J. 1986. A preliminary analysis of the suppressive effects of denatonium saccharide.Bull. Psychon. Soc. 24:229–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, S.F., Grover, C.A., Erickson, C.A., Miller, L.A., andBowman, J.A. 1987. Analyzing aversiveness of denatonium saccharide and quinine in rats.Percept. Mot. Skills. 64:1215–1222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeland, W.J., andJanzen, D.J. 1974. Strategies in herbivory by mammals: The role of plant secondary compounds.Am. Nat. 108:269–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J., andHankins, W.G. 1975. The evolution of bitter and the acquisition of toxiphobia, pp. 39–45,in D.A. Denton and J.P. Coughlin (eds.). Olfaction and Taste: Fifth International Symposium. Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glendinning, J.I. 1992. Effectiveness of cardenolides as feeding deterrents toPeromyscus mice.J. Chem. Ecol. 18:1559–1575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glendinning, J.I., Brower, L.P., andMontgomery, C.A. 1990. Responses of three mouse species to deterrent chemicals in the monarch butterfly. I. Taste and toxicity tests using artificial diets laced with digitoxin or monocrotaline.Chemoecology. 1:114–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.W. 1978. Taste responses in wild and domestic guinea-pigs.Physiol. Behav. 20:579–588.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.W., andLabows, J.N. 1979. Conditioned aversion, bitter taste and the avoidance of natural toxicants in wild guinea pigs.Physiol. Behav. 22:173–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, W.W., Beauchamp, G.K., andKare, M.R. 1978. Progress in animal flavor research, pp. 1–20,in R.W. Bullard (ed.). Flavor Chemistry of Animal Foods. American Chemical Society Symposium 67. American Chemical Society, Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley, W.M., Theis, J., Davis, S.F., Richard, M.M., andGrover, C.M. 1987. Effects of denatonium saccharide on the drinking behavior of the grasshopper mouse (Onychomys leucogaster).Bull. Psychon. Soc. 25:17–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindroth, R.L. 1988. Adaptations of mammalian herbivores to plant chemical defenses, pp. 415–445,in K.C. Spencer (ed.). Chemical Mediation of Coevolution. Academic Press, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maga, J.A. 1990. Compound structure versus bitter taste, pp. 35–48,in R.L. Rouseff (ed.). Bitterness in Foods and Beverages. Elsevier, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J.R., Reidinger, R.F., andStewart, C.N. 1985. Profiling, mimicking, and masking the flavor of a selected rodenticide.Physiol. Behav. 35:127–134.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolte, D.L., Mason, J.R., andClark, L. 1993. Avoidance of bird repellents by mice (Mus musculus).J. Chem. Ecol. 19:427–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Provenza, F.D., Burritt, E.A., Clausan, T.P., Bryant, J.P., Reichardt, P.B., andDistal, R.A. 1990. Conditioned flavor aversion: A mechanism for goats to avoid condensed tannins in blackbrush.Am. Nat. 136:811–828.

    Google Scholar 

  • Provenza, F.D., Lynch, J.J., Burritt, E.A., andScott, C.B. 1993. How goats learn to distinguish between novel foods that differ in postingestive consequences.J. Chem. Ecol. Submitted.

  • Winer, B.G. 1971. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design. McGraw-Hill, New York, 907 pp.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Post doctoral fellow at the Monell Chemical Senses Center at the time of the experiment.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nolte, D.L., Russell Mason, J. & Lewis, S.L. Tolerance of bitter compounds by an herbivore,Cavia porcellus . J Chem Ecol 20, 303–308 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02064438

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02064438

Key Words

Navigation