Skip to main content
Log in

Listening Comprehension and Working Memory Are Impaired in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Irrespective of Language Impairment

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated listening comprehension and working memory abilities in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), presenting with and without language impairments (LI). A 4-group design classified a community sample (n = 77) of boys aged 9–12 into ADHD, ADHD + LI, LI, and Normal groups. Children completed tests of basic language and cognitive skills, verbal and spatial working memory, and passage-level listening comprehension. Multivariate analyses and post hoc comparisons indicated that ADHD children who did not have co-occurring LI comprehended factual information from spoken passages as well as normal children, but were poorer at comprehending inferences and monitoring comprehension of instructions. ADHD children did not differ from normal children in verbal span, but showed significantly poorer verbal working memory, spatial span, and spatial working memory. The ADHD + LI and LI groups were most impaired in listening comprehension and working memory performance, but did not differ from each other. Listening comprehension skills were significantly correlated with both verbal and spatial working memory, and parent–teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Findings that children with ADHD but no LI showed subtle higher-level listening comprehension deficits have implications for both current diagnostic practices and conceptualizations of ADHD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, W., & Sheslow, D. (1990). Wide range assessment of memory and llearning. Wilmington, Delaware: Wide Rang.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unified theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bashir, A., & Scavuzzo, A. (1992). Children with language disorders: Natural history and academic success. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 53–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beitchman, J. H., Wilson, B., Johnson, C. J., Atkinson, L., Young, A., Adlaf, E., et al. (2001). Fourteen year follow-up of speech/language impaired and control children: Psychiatric outcome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40, 75–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brock, S. E., & Knapp, P. K. (1996). Reading comprehension abilities of children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 173–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. E. (Ed.). (2000). Attention deficit disorders and comorbidities in children, adolescents, and adults, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2000). Investigating the causes of reading comprehension failure: The comprehension–age match design. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1991). Psychiatric and developmental disorders in children with communication disorder. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. (1995). Dale Chall Readability—Revisited. The Dale Chall Readability Formula. New York: Brookline Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, M. J. (1997). Children's Memory Scale. San Antonio IX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., Menna, R., Vallance, D., Barwick, M., Im, N., & Horodetsky, N. (1998). Language, social cognitive processing, and behavioral characteristics of psychiatrically disturbed children with previously identified and unsuspected language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(6), 853–864.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N., Davine, M., Horodetsky, N., Lipsett, L. & Isaacson, L. (1993). Unsuspected language impairment in psychiatrically disturbed children: Prevalence and language and behavioral characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 595–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., Barwick, M., Horodetsky, N., Vallance, D., Im, N. (1998). Language, achievement, and cognitive processing in psychiatrically disturbed children with previously identified and unidentified language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry, 39 865–877.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, N. J., Vallance, D. D., Barwick, M., Im, N., Menna, R., and Horodezky, N., et al. (2000). The interface between ADHD and language impairment: An examination of language, achievement, and cognitive processing. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 353–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners Rating Scales—Revised. Technical manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, V. I. (1999). Cognitive control processes in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. In H. C. Quay & A. E. Hogan (Eds.), Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders (pp. 105–138). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duff, S. J., & Hampson, E. (2001). A sex difference on a novel spatial working memory task in humans. Brain and Cognition, 47, 470–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2000). Differential roles for visuospatial and verbal working memory in situation model construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 61–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2000). Working memory deficits in children with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 177–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, S., & Goudvis, A. (2000). Strategies that work. Teaching comprehension to enhance understanding. York, Maine: Stenhouse.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., Young, A., Escobar, M., Atkinson, L., Wilson, B., et al. (1999). Fourteen year follow-up of children with and without speech/language impairments: Speech–language stability and outcomes. Journal of Speech-Language Hearing Research, 42, 744–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: individual difference in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karatekin, C., & Asarnow, R. F. (1998). Working memory in childhood onset schizo-phrenia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 80, 165–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempton, S., Vance, A., Maruff, E., Luk, E., Costin, C., & Pantelis, C. (1999). Executive function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: stimulant medication and better executive function performance in children. Psychological Medicine 29, 527–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension. A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., & Sanchez, R. P. (1998). Story comprehension in children with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 163–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., Sanchez, R. P., van den Brock, P., Baer, S., Hooks, K., et al. (2000). Comprehension of televised stories in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 321–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don't understand. Elementary school children's awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643–655.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martinussen, R., & McInnes, A. (2001). Visual spatial memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Subtype differences and stimulant effects. Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, Minneapolis, Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Toward unified theories of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 442–473). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. (1999). Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 139–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oram, J., Fine, J., & Tannock, R. (1999). Assessing the language of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 8, 72–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palladino, P., Cornoldi, C., de Beni, R., & Pazzaglia, F. (2001). Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 29, 344–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits in subject-ordered tasks after frontal and temporal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prizant, B. M., Audet, L. R., Burke, G. M., Hummel, L. J., Maher, S. R., & Theadore, G. (1990) Communication disorders and emotional/behavioral disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Speech Language Hearing Research, 55, 179–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purvis, K., & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 133–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahakian, B. J., & Owen, A. M. (1992). Computerized assessment in neuropsychiatry using CANTAB. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 85, 399–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—III, San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading comprehension and working memory in learning disabled readers: Is the phonological loop more important than the executive system? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., & Brown, T. E. (2000). Attention deficit disorders with learning disorders in children and adolescents. In T. E. Brown (Ed.), Attention deficit disorders and comorbidities in children, adolescents, and adults, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Hum, M., Masellis, M., Humphries, T., & Schachar, R. (2000). Children's Academic Performance, Attention, Behavior, and Learning Evaluation: Teacher Telephone Interview (CAPABLE-TTI). Presented at the annual conference of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, New York City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., & Schachar, R. (1996). Executive dysfunction as an underlying mechanism of behavior and language problems in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In J. Beitchman, N. Cohen, M. M. Konstantearas, and R. Tannock. (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavour disorders (pp. 128–155). Cambridge: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannock, R., Purvis, K. L., & Schachar, R. (1993). Narrative abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 21, 103–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tirosh, E., & Cohen, A. (1998). Language deficit with attention-deficit disorder: A prevalent comorbidity. Journal of Child Neurology, 13, 493–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallance, D. D., Im, N., & Cohen, N. J. (1999). Discourse deficits associated with psychiatric disorders and with language impairments in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 693–704.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warr-Leeper, G., Wright, N. A., & Mack, A. (1994). Language disabilities of antisocial boys in residential treatment. Behavior Disorders, 19, 159–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third edition, San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westby, C. (1991). Assessing and remediating text comprehension problems. In A. G. Kahmi & H. W. Catts (Eds.), reading disabilities. A developmental language perspective (pp. 199–260). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D., Stott, C. M., Goodyer, I. M., & Sahakian, B. J. (2000). Specific language development with or without hyperactivity: Neuropsychological evidence for frontostriatal dysfunction. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 42, 368–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987).Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests—Revised, Austin, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ylvisaker, M., & DeBonis, D. (2000). Executive function impairment in adolescence: TBI and ADHD. Topics in Language Disorders, 20, 29–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, S. S. (1988). Production deficiencies in elicited language but not in the spontaneous verbalizations of hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 657–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zentall, S. S. (1993). Research on the educational implications of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Exceptional Children, 60, 143–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rosemary Tannock.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McInnes, A., Humphries, T., Hogg-Johnson, S. et al. Listening Comprehension and Working Memory Are Impaired in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Irrespective of Language Impairment. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31, 427–443 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023895602957

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023895602957

Navigation