Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a severely impairing mental disorder with a lifetime prevalence of approximately 1–3% (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Fontenelle et al., 2006; Guzick et al., 2017; Lieb et al., 2019). It is characterized by recurrent intrusive and unwanted thoughts (obsessions) that are typically followed by mental acts or observable behaviors (compulsions) aimed at reducing the distress or perceived danger associated with these obsessions (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Previous studies suggest that a ruminative thinking style (RTS) is positively associated with obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptom severity (e.g., Dar & Iqbal, 2015; Wahl et al., 2011). An RTS has been defined as “a mode of responding to distress that involves repetitively and passively focusing on symptoms of distress and on the possible causes and consequences of these symptoms” (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008, p. 400). This concept originated from the literature on depression and the associations between an RTS and symptoms of depression, and many other mental disorders have been investigated extensively (for overviews, see Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008).

Multiple studies have supported positive associations of an RTS with OC symptom severity. Between-group comparisons have shown a stronger self-reported RTS in individuals diagnosed with OCD compared to those diagnosed with a generalized anxiety disorder (Dar & Iqbal, 2015; however, note that Armstrong et al., 2011 did not find such differences) and those without any diagnosis of an Axis I disorder from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Armstrong et al., 2011; Koch & Exner, 2015). Furthermore, an RTS has been positively associated with OC symptom severity in students and the general population (Grisham & Williams, 2009; Jungmann et al., 2016; Shaw et al., 2017; Wahl et al., 2011) as well as in heterogeneous clinical samples of individuals diagnosed with a variety of mental disorders (e.g., anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, and OCD; Dar & Iqbal, 2015; Raines et al., 2017). Recently, Wei et al. (2020) demonstrated this association in a treatment-receiving sample of adolescents and young adults diagnosed with OCD, although these results should be interpreted with caution due to the study’s use of an unvalidated two-item measure of an RTS. Two recent experimental studies have shown an RTS to maintain the urge to neutralize unwanted intrusive thoughts (a subclinical analog to the urge to perform compulsions in response to unwanted intrusions; Kollárik et al., 2020; Wahl et al., 2019). These experimental studies suggest that an RTS might not only be associated with OC symptom severity but might also contribute to its maintenance. Although this literature provides strong support for a positive association of an RTS and OC symptom severity, no study has so far investigated this association in a sample consisting exclusively of individuals with a diagnosis of OCD using a well-established RTS measure. Replicating these results in such a sample would further strengthen the evidence for this association.

Several cognitive mechanisms that might explain a possible functional relationship between an RTS and obsessive thoughts and compulsions have been proposed. For instance, an RTS may facilitate dysfunctional interpretations of intrusive thoughts (Raines et al., 2017; Wahl et al., 2019) that are seen as central to the development and maintenance of OC symptoms in current models of OCD (e.g., Rachman, 1998). However, the high comorbidity of OCD with depression and anxiety disorders (Baer et al., 2017; Lieb et al., 2019) suggests an alternative explanation for the relationship between an RTS and the severity of OC symptoms: It may be due to both constructs’ associations with depressive and anxiety symptom severity. This explanation is supported by both depressive and anxiety symptom severity showing medium to large positive associations with OC symptom severity (e.g., Abramowitz & Deacon, 2006; Clark et al., 2005; Goodman et al., 1989a; Tellawi et al., 2016) as well as with an RTS (e.g., Merino et al., 2014; Merino et al., 2016; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Rogers et al., 2019; Treynor et al., 2003; Yang et al., 2014).

If the association between an RTS and OC symptom severity can be accounted for by depressive and anxiety symptom severity, then the association should disappear when these confounders are statistically controlled for. So far, only a few studies have statistically controlled for these variables. Wahl et al. (2011) and Raines et al. (2017) conducted studies on samples of students and individuals diagnosed with various mental disorders, respectively. In these studies, an RTS was positively associated with the severity of some, but not all, OC symptoms when controlling for depressive symptom severity or negative affect. Only two studies have reported associations between an RTS and OC symptom severity while controlling for both depressive and anxiety symptom severity at the same time. Grisham and Williams (2009) demonstrated a moderate positive relationship of an RTS with OC symptom severity when controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity in a student sample. Shaw et al. (2017) conducted an online study with a sample of individuals who had mostly (83%) not reported previous diagnoses of mental disorders. In their study, only one of the OC symptom subscales, responsibility for harm, showed a small positive association with an RTS after controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity. The associations of an RTS with the other three subscales (contamination, symmetry, and unacceptable thoughts) as well as total OC symptom severity were not significant after controlling for these possible confounders. To summarize, although there is conflicting evidence, previous studies have demonstrated associations of an RTS with some, but not all, aspects of OC symptom severity when controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity. However, none of the previous studies investigated these associations in a sample of individuals diagnosed with OCD. If they can be replicated with individuals diagnosed with OCD, this may help inform cognitive-behavioral models and optimal treatment of OCD. For example, an RTS may emerge as an additional maladaptive response to OC symptoms that maintains these symptoms in the long term (as suggested by Kollárik et al., 2020; Wahl et al., 2019, in press). If this is the case, therapists and individuals with OCD could work collaboratively on identifying an RTS as a potentially maladaptive response to OC symptoms as part of the case conceptualization in cognitive-behavioral therapy, and this assumption would be tested in later behavioral experiments. Moreover, if an RTS maintains OC symptoms, then eventually cognitive-behavioral therapy could be supplemented by specific interventions that target an RTS, for example, an adapted version of rumination-focused cognitive-behavioral therapy (RFCBT; Watkins, 2016), mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Külz et al., 2019), or interpretive bias modification (Hirsch et al., 2020).

Additionally, none of the previous studies used the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS; Goodman et al., 1989b). The Y-BOCS is an interview measure of OC symptom severity that is generally considered the gold standard (e.g., Grabill et al., 2008). Compared to the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory—Revised (OCI-R; Foa et al., 2002; used by Grisham & Williams, 2009), which focuses on the distress associated with OC symptoms, the Y-BOCS also measures additional aspects of OC symptom severity, such as interference from or control over OC symptoms (Abramowitz & Deacon, 2006). Furthermore, when an RTS is assessed with a self-report questionnaire, using an interview measure of OC symptom severity has the additional advantage of reducing the possibility that associations with an RTS are due to common method variance (e.g., Podsakoff et al., 2003).

This study aimed to investigate the association of an RTS with OC symptom severity with and without controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity in a sample of individuals with OCD. We extended previous studies by using an interview measure (Y-BOCS) in addition to a questionnaire (OCI-R) to assess OC symptom severity. First, we hypothesized that an RTS would be positively associated with both (1a) a questionnaire and (1b) an interview measure of OC symptom severity. Second, we hypothesized that an RTS would remain positively associated with (2a) the questionnaire and (2b) the interview measure of OC symptom severity after depressive and anxiety symptom severity were accounted for. Additionally, we used exploratory analyses to investigate the associations of an RTS with the subscales of both OC symptom severity measures, with and without controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

All participants were recruited and tested in six inpatient and outpatient clinics in Germany as part of the three studies investigating psychological mechanisms in OCD. Therefore, three slightly different subsamples were aggregated in this study. In total, data from N = 140 individuals diagnosed with OCD were included. Criteria for all diagnoses in this study were based on the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Exclusion criteria for all participants were current substance abuse or dependence, current psychotic symptoms, or current suicidal intent. Additionally, in Subsample 1, but not the other subsamples, individuals with diagnoses of major depressive disorder or generalized anxiety disorder within the past 12 months were excluded from study participation.

All participants were tested within the first 2 weeks of treatment. Subsamples 1 and 2 completed identical measures of all variables. Subsample 3 also completed these measures, but no interview measure of OC symptom severity was administered. Experienced clinicians or trained graduate students assessed the participants’ OCD and comorbid diagnoses using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; Wittchen et al., 1997). During the same session, they measured OC symptom severity using the Y-BOCS (Goodman et al., 1989b). Furthermore, each participant completed the questionnaires detailed in the Materials section during the same session. All participants gave their written informed consent. The participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. The three Subsamples did not differ with regard to age, F(2, 134) = 0.33, p = .72, or gender, χ2(2) = 0.86, p = .65. Because of the different exclusion criteria for Subsample 1 compared to the other two subsamples, we analyzed current comorbidities of anxiety and major depressive disorders. The subsamples did not differ regarding their levels of current anxiety disorder comorbidities, χ2(2) = 4.28, p = .12, but they differed with regard to their concurrent comorbidities with major depressive disorder, χ2(2) = 24.20, p < .001. Regarding RTS, depressive, anxiety, and OC symptom severity, we tested for differences between the subsamples using analyses of variance and Gabriel’s post hoc tests or, where their assumptions were violated, Kruskal-Wallis and Games-Howell tests. The subsamples did not differ with regard to the questionnaire measures of OC symptom severity or anxiety symptom severity. However, they showed different levels to which they adopted an RTS and different levels of depressive symptom severity, and they differed in the interview measure of OC symptom severity.

Table 1 Participant characteristics of the total sample and subsamples

Materials

RTS Assessment

To assess the participants’ RTS, we employed the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991; German version by Huffziger & Kühner, 2012). The RRS is a questionnaire measuring typical ruminative responses to dysphoric mood with three subscales (Treynor et al., 2003). We used only the brooding subscale (Treynor et al., 2003) as a measure of an RTS that is not confounded with the assessment of depressive symptoms. The brooding subscale consists of five items and has shown adequate psychometric properties in both its English and German versions (Huffziger & Kühner, 2012; Treynor et al., 2003). In our study, the internal consistency of the brooding subscale was acceptable, with Cronbach’s α = .73.

Assessment of OC Symptom Severity

We assessed OC symptom severity in two ways. First, we employed the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory—Revised (OCI-R; Foa et al., 2002; German version by Gönner et al., 2008), an 18-item questionnaire measuring OC symptom severity during the past month. OC symptom severity is operationalized by the distress caused by OC symptoms (Foa et al., 2002). The questionnaire comprises six subscales: washing, checking, ordering, obsessing, hoarding, and neutralizing. The OCI-R has high reliability and good convergent validity (Foa et al., 2002; Gönner et al., 2008). Although the strength of its associations with measures of depressive symptom severity varies, its discriminant validity is generally acceptable (Abramowitz & Deacon, 2006; Foa et al., 2002; Gönner et al., 2008). In the present study, the OCI-R total score had high internal consistency, with Cronbach’s α = .82.

Second, we used the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS; Goodman et al., 1989b; German version by Hand & Büttner-Westphal, 1991), which is a semistructured interview assessing the severity of OC symptoms during the past week. The Y-BOCS comprises two subscales—obsessions and compulsions—with five items each. In this study, a total score of all 10 items was used to measure obsession and compulsion severity in addition to the two subscales. The Y-BOCS has high reliability (Goodman et al., 1989b; Jacobsen et al., 2003; Woody et al., 1995) and good convergent validity (Abramowitz et al., 2010; Gönner et al., 2008). However, it can be criticized for its high correlations with measures of depression (Clark et al., 2005; Goodman et al., 1989a; Rees et al., 2014). In contrast to the operationalization in the OCI-R, OC symptom severity is operationalized more globally in the Y-BOCS, which measures not only the distress associated with but also the interference from, resistance against, control over, and time spent on obsessions and compulsions (Abramowitz & Deacon, 2006).

Assessment of Depressive and Anxiety Symptom Severity

We assessed depressive symptom severity during the past week using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1961; German version by Hautzinger et al., 1995), which consists of 21 items. Its high reliability and validity have been shown in numerous studies (Beck et al., 1988b; Hautzinger et al., 1995). In this study, the BDI showed excellent internal consistency, with Cronbach’s α = .90.

Anxiety symptom severity during the past week was measured using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck et al., 1988a; German version by Margraf & Ehlers, 2007), a 21-item questionnaire. The BAI has high reliability and good validity (e.g., Beck et al., 1988a; Margraf & Ehlers, 2007). In the present study, its internal consistency was excellent, with Cronbach’s α = .91.

Analyses

All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 26.0, and a significance level of α = 5%. We examined the distribution of the main variables using visual inspection of histograms and Q-Q plots as well as tests of normality (Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilk). Because the values for the questionnaire measures of OC, depressive, and anxiety symptom severity were nonnormally distributed (see the online supplement for the variables’ skew and kurtosis statistics), we applied a square-root transformation to these three variables before regression analyses. After we applied these transformations, the assumptions for all analyses were met. Because the three subsamples differed with regard to RTS, depressive symptom severity, and the interview measure of OC symptom severity, we controlled for subsamples in our analyses, using dummy coding where applicable. We investigated Hypothesis 1a and b by conducting linear regression analyses with an RTS predicting each measure of OC symptom severity and controlling for subsamples. For Hypothesis 2a and b, we extended the regression models to include the predictors depressive and anxiety symptom severity. For comparison with previous studies, we report partial correlation coefficients (prs) for the association between each predictor and the outcome in addition to regression coefficients in the regression models. For the exploratory analyses, we repeated the regression models from our Hypotheses 1 and 2 with the subscales of each OC symptom severity measure (except for OCI-R hoarding, because hoarding is classified as a separate disorder in the DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) as outcomes. Although we also controlled for subsamples in these analyses, for brevity, we do not report statistics for these predictors in the exploratory analyses. As additional information, we report zero-order correlations between the main variables in the online supplement. We checked for univariate outliers using boxplots and scatterplots and for multivariate outliers using Mahalanobis distance (cutoffs based on Kline, 2016). Two multivariate outliers emerged and closer examination of the participants showed discrepancies on the Y-BOCS, but otherwise the participants’ characteristics seemed to fit the target population. We excluded these two participants from all analyses using the Y-BOCS subscales and total scores, and when we repeated the outlier analyses, no multivariate outliers emerged.

Results

The RTS showed positive associations with both the questionnaire and the interview measure of OC symptom severity when only subsamples were controlled for (see Model 1 in Table 2). When we additionally controlled for depressive and anxiety symptom severity (see Model 2 in Table 2), the RTS continued to predict the questionnaire-based measure of OC symptom severity. However, the association between the RTS and the interview measure of OC symptom severity was not retained after depressive and anxiety symptom severity were controlled for.

Table 2 RTS and depressive and anxiety symptom severity predicting two measures of OC symptom severity

Tables 3 and 4 present exploratory analyses for the associations of the RTS with OCI-R and Y-BOCS subscales. Regarding the OCI-R subscales, exploratory analyses showed that the highest association with an RTS, controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, emerged for ordering (Model 2 in Table 3). The next highest effect size, controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, emerged for OCI-R mental neutralizing. Model 2 for OCI-R obsessing showed a similar effect size to OCI-R mental neutralizing but did not quite reach significance. Regarding the Y-BOCS subscales, exploratory analyses demonstrated that neither the obsessions nor the compulsions subscale remained associated with an RTS when depressive and anxiety symptom severity were controlled for (Model 2 in Table 4).

Table 3 Exploratory analyses of the RTS and depressive and anxiety symptom severity predicting the OC symptom severity questionnaire measure subscales, controlling for subsamples
Table 4 Exploratory analyses of the RTS and depressive and anxiety symptom severity predicting the OC symptom severity interview measure subscales, controlling for subsamples

Discussion

The present study investigated the association between an RTS and OC symptom severity, controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, in individuals diagnosed with OCD. Before depressive and anxiety symptom severity were controlled for, the RTS were positively associated with two different measures of OC symptom severity (OCI-R and Y-BOCS, Foa et al., 2002; Goodman et al., 1989b). These findings replicate those reported in previous studies (Dar & Iqbal, 2015; Grisham & Williams, 2009; Jungmann et al., 2016; Raines et al., 2017; Shaw et al., 2017; Wahl et al., 2011; Wei et al., 2020) in a sample consisting exclusively of individuals diagnosed with OCD. The effect sizes in our study tended to be lower than those reported by previous studies using similar measures (for the questionnaire measure: pr = .45, cf. r = .58 in Grisham & Williams, 2009; for the interview measure: β = .28, cf. β = .55 in Wei et al., 2020). Our results provide further evidence on the association between an RTS and OC symptoms in the population that is most relevant for potential conceptual and clinical implications.

When statistically controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, the RTS remained positively associated with the questionnaire-based measure of OC symptom severity, supporting Hypothesis 2a. The results for this measure replicate those of previous studies on students and individuals diagnosed with various mental disorders (Grisham & Williams, 2009; Raines et al., 2017; Wahl et al., 2011) in individuals diagnosed with OCD. The effect size in our study (pr = .25) is in between those of previous studies using similar covariates (pr = .34, Grisham & Williams, 2009; pr = .12, Shaw et al., 2017). Exploratory analyses show that the RTS was associated with the ordering and mental neutralizing subscales, but not the other subscales of the questionnaire measure of OC symptom severity when controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity. The RTS did not retain an association with the interview measure of OC symptom severity when controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, and thus Hypothesis 2b was not supported. Similarly, exploratory analyses show that when depressive and anxiety symptom severity were controlled for, the RTS was not associated with either subscale of the interview measure of OC symptom severity.

If future studies can replicate the results for Hypothesis 2a, this would be consistent with the idea that an RTS and OC symptom severity are related independent of both variables’ associations with depressive and anxiety severity. The relation between an RTS and OC symptom severity could be explained by an RTS facilitating dysfunctional appraisals of intrusive thoughts (Raines et al., 2017; Wahl et al., 2019). For instance, thinking about one’s obsessions with an RTS (e.g., “why do I, of all people, experience these impulses to stab someone?”) may facilitate misinterpretations of these obsessions as disclosing some hidden personal characteristic (e.g., “These impulses must mean that I am a dangerous person”). Such dysfunctional interpretations are a presumed key factor in the development and maintenance of OC symptoms (Rachman, 1998). Alternatively, ruminating on intrusive thoughts might increase the accessibility of these thoughts and thus lead individuals to experience intrusive thoughts more frequently (Grisham & Williams, 2009). Further studies with experimental or prospective designs could allow a closer examination of the underlying mechanisms of how an RTS may affect OC symptoms and vice versa. Ultimately, current cognitive-behavioral models of OCD (e.g., Salkovskis, 1985) may need to be extended by including the role of an RTS in the maintenance of OCD.

Depending on future studies, it might be advisable to include supplemental interventions that directly target an RTS in OCD treatment. Our preliminary exploratory analyses suggest that such interventions may be especially warranted for individuals predominantly experiencing ordering and mental neutralizing symptoms, but further replications are needed. Specific supplemental interventions could include adapted versions of RFCBT that aim, among other things, to interrupt the habitual use of an RTS (Watkins, 2016); mindfulness-based cognitive therapy that increases a nonjudgmental, accepting attitude toward OC symptoms (Külz et al., 2019); or interpretative bias modification that aims to modify the interpretation of potentially ambiguous situations that can be common starting points for excessive ruminative thinking patterns (Hirsch et al., 2020). For example, some of the components of RFCBT could be applied to an RTS in individuals diagnosed with OCD without any or with only small changes. Using functional analysis and self-monitoring, individuals with OCD could learn to identify situations that result in a strong tendency to adopt an RTS. However, other components of RFCBT might have to be tailored to individuals with OCD: While individuals diagnosed with depression might subsequently work on finding alternative, more helpful behaviors or thoughts to replace an RTS, in individuals with OCD, exposure to the original obsessions would have to be considered as a further, potentially more adaptive alternative.

However, if future studies replicate our results for Hypothesis 2b and fail to demonstrate associations between an RTS and OC symptom severity when controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, it seems likely that the high comorbidity of depression and/or anxiety disorders with OCD (Baer et al., 2017; Lieb et al., 2019) is the main reason that an RTS and OC symptom severity are associated. This high comorbidity reflects the positive associations of OC symptom severity with both depressive and anxiety symptom severity (e.g., Clark et al., 2005; Tellawi et al., 2016). At the same time, the latter two constructs are also positively associated with an RTS (e.g., Merino et al., 2016; Treynor et al., 2003). Thus, depressive and anxiety symptom severity could potentially act as confounders that lead to the association between an RTS and OC symptom severity.

The different findings for the two measures of OC symptom severity might be explained in various ways. First, they might be due to the questionnaire (OCI-R) assessing different aspects of OC symptom severity than the interview (Y-BOCS). The OCI-R assesses only the distress associated with symptoms of OCD, whereas the Y-BOCS measures more global OC symptom severity including the time spent on, interference from, distress of, resistance against, and control over OC symptoms (Abramowitz & Deacon, 2006). Consistent with this idea, previous studies show associations between the OCI-R and Y-BOCS (e.g., r = .41, Abramowitz & Deacon, 2006; r = .45, Deacon & Abramowitz, 2005; r = .43, Gönner et al., 2008) that stay below what might be considered good convergent validity in this context (McGuire et al., 2017). It is possible that an RTS is particularly associated with the distress caused by some OC symptoms but no other aspects such as how time-consuming OC symptoms are. This may also explain why after controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity, Grisham and Williams (2009) reported an association of an RTS with the OCI-R while Shaw et al. (2017) did not find an RTS to be related to the overall score of the Dimensional Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (DOCS; Abramowitz et al., 2010). Similar to the Y-BOCS, the DOCS assesses OC symptom severity more globally. Alternatively, the variance of the OCI-R explained by the RTS could have been at least partly caused by common method variance (e.g., Podsakoff et al., 2003) because these variables were both measured using questionnaires, whereas the Y-BOCS is an interview measure. Future studies should investigate whether these associations are retained when OC symptom severity is assessed using different measures. For instance, comparing the association of an RTS with scores on the DOCS to associations of an RTS with scores on the OCI-R and Y-BOCS may illuminate possible reasons for the differing results in our study further.

Limitations of the present study include the fact that our sample for analyzing the association of an RTS with the interview measure of OC symptom severity was relatively small, which may have limited our power to detect small to medium associations. Furthermore, on average, participants in our study had lower comorbidities (Baer et al., 2017; Quarantini et al., 2011; Torres et al., 2006) and lower scores on the measure of depressive symptom severity (cf. Clayton et al., 1999; MacDonald et al., 1999) than have previously been reported for other OCD samples. This means that our study’s results may not be transferable to those of other samples of individuals diagnosed with OCD. Future studies should investigate whether our results can be replicated with different and larger samples of individuals diagnosed with OCD.

To conclude, our study revealed that in individuals diagnosed with OCD, an RTS was associated with a questionnaire-based but not an interview-based measure of OC symptom severity after controlling for depressive and anxiety symptom severity. Further studies should investigate more closely the nature of the relationship of an RTS with the distress associated with OC symptoms and also target the diverging results of the questionnaire and the interview measure of OC symptom severity. Ultimately, psychological treatments of OCD may be supplemented by interventions that specifically target an RTS, as RFCBT (Watkins, 2016) does in the context of major depressive disorder.