Skip to main content
Log in

Language Acquisition and EcoDevo Processes: The Case of the Lexicon-Syntax Interface

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Ecological developmental biology (EcoDevo) considers the phenotype as actively produced through an environmentally informed process of individual development, rather than predetermined by the genotype. Accordingly, the genotype is viewed as one among many interactants that contribute formative elements; it is understood to do so no differently from the way other organism-internal and environmental resources do. Although the EcoDevo approach is evidently particularly apt to inform approaches to human development, which mostly takes shape in rich cultural environments, it is remarkable that, at least within some highly influential circles of the linguistic sciences, the study of many distinctive human cognitive traits including language remains strongly anchored to a preformationist stance. This article brings an EcoDevo approach to the biologically focused study of language to argue that an aspect of language design commonly assumed to be “blueprinted,” namely, the lexicon-syntax interface, may instead be explained as a plastic effect of ongoing developmental processes in the mind of the child, in which designated aspects of the environment fulfill a key constructive role.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alberch P (1989) The logic of monsters: evidence for internal constraint in development and evolution. Geobios 12(mémoire spécial):21–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Alberch P (1991a) From genes to phenotype: dynamical systems and evolvability. Genetica 84:5–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Alberch P (1991b) Del gen al fenotipo: Sistemas dinámicos y evolución morfológica. Rev Esp Paleontolog (número extraordinario):13–19

  • Amundson R (2001) Adaptation and development: on the lack of common ground. In: Orzack S, Sober E (eds) Adaptationism and optimality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 303–334

    Google Scholar 

  • Amundson R (2005) The changing role of the embryo in evolutionary thought. Roots of Evo-Devo. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld M, Rosenberg E, Obermann M, Gasparini D (1998) Lexical acceleration coincides with the onset of combinatorial speech. First Lang 18:165–184

    Google Scholar 

  • Balari S, Lorenzo G (2008) Pere Alberch’s developmental morphospaces and the evolution of cognition. Biol Theory 3:297–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Balari S, Lorenzo G (2013) Computational phenotypes. Towards an evolutionary developmental biolinguistics. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Balari S, Lorenzo G (2015) It is an organ, it is new, but it is not a new organ. Conceptualizing language from a homological perspective. Front Ecol Evol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2015.00058

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balari S, Lorenzo G (2018) The internal, the external and the hybrid: the state of the art and a new characterization of language as a natural object. Glossa. J Gen Linguist. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.330

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates E, Goodman JC (1997) On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. Lang Cogn Process 12:507–584

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates E, Goodman JC (1999) On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In: MacWhinney B (ed) The Emergence of Language. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 29–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates E, Brethenton I, Snyder L (1988) From first words to grammar: individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson P, Gluckman P (2011) Plasticity, robustness, and evolution. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel W, Richardson RC (2010) Discovering complexity. Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2016) Why only us. Language and evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick RC, Beckers GJL, Okanoya K, Bolhuis JJ (2012) A bird’s eye view of human language evolution. Front Evol Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2012.00005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berwick RC, Friederici AD, Chomsky N, Bolhuis JJ (2013) Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends Cogn Sci 7:89–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton D (2014) More than nature needs. Language, mind, and evolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolhuis JJ, Tattersall I, Chomsky N, Berwick RC (2014) How could language have evolved? PLoS Biol 12:e1001934. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonduriansky R, Day T (2018) Extended heredity. A new understanding of inheritance and evolution. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonner JT (ed) (1982) Evolution and development. Report of the Dahlem workshop on evolution and development, Berlin 1981, May 10–15. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandon RN (1990) Adaptation and environment. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert M, Stevens M, Mandera P, Keuleers E (2016) How many words do we know? Practical estimates of vocabulary size dependent on word definition, the degree of language input and the participant’s age. Front Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll SB (2008) Evo-Devo and an expanding evolutionary synthesis: a genetic theory of morphological evolution. Cell 134:25–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu L, Gilbert SF (2015) The birth of the holobiont: multi-species birthing through mutual scaffolding and niche construction. Biosemiotics 8:191–210

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1956a) Three models for the description of language. IRE Trans Inf Theory 2:113–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1956b) On the limits of finite-state description. Q Prog Rep 42:64–65

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1965) Aspect of the theory of syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1975) Reflections on language. Pantheon, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1980) Rules and representations. Columbia University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1986) Knowledge of language. Its origins, nature and use. Praeger, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1995) The minimalist program. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2000) Minimalist inquiries: the framework. In: Martin R, Michaels D, Uriagereka J (eds) Step by step. Essays in honor of Howard Lasnik. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 89–155

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2005) Three factors in language design. Linguist Inq 36:1–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2007) Approaching UG from below. In: Sauerland U, Gärtner HM (eds) Interfaces + recursion = language? Chomsky’s minimalism and the view from syntax-semantics. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 1–19

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2010) Some simple evo devo theses: how true might they be for language. In: Larson RK, Déprez V, Yamakido H (eds) The evolution of language. Biolinguistic perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 199–210

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2013) Problems of projection. Lingua 130:33–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (2016) What kinds of creatures are we? Columbia University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N, Hauser MD, Fitch WT (2005) Appendix. The minimalist program. http://arti.vub.ac.be/cursus/2005-2006/mwo/03b-mp.pdf

  • Christiansen MH, Chater N (2016) Creating language. Integrating development, acquisition, and processing. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis M (2011) The recursive mind. The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L, Tooby J (1997) Dissecting the computational architecture of social interference mechanisms. In: Bock GR, Cardew G (eds) Characterizing human psychological adaptations. John Wiley, Chichester, pp 132–161

    Google Scholar 

  • Danchin É, Charmantier A, Champagne FA, Mesoudi A, Pujol B, Blanchet S (2011) Beyond DNA: integrating inclusive inheritance into an extended theory of evolution. Nat Rev Genet 12:475–486

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson EH (1982) Evolutionary change in genomic regulatory organization: speculations on the origins of novel biological structure. In: Bonner JT (ed) Evolution and development. Report of the Dahlem workshop on evolution and development, Berlin 1981, May 10–15. Springer, Berlin, pp 65–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson EH (1990) How embryos work: a comparative view of diverse modes of cell fate specification. Development 108:365–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson EH (2006) The regulatory genome. Gene regulatory networks in development and evolution. Academic Press, San Diego

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson EH, McClay DR, Hood L (2003) Regulatory gene networks and the properties of the developmental process. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:1475–1480

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon TW (1997) The symbolic species. The co-evolution of language and the brain. Norton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Desmond A, Moore J (2009) Darwin. Penguin, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dick AS, Müller U (eds) (2018) Advancing developmental science. philosophy, theory, and method. Routledge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dionne G, Gale P, Boivin M, Plomin R (2003) Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Dev 17:33–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon JA, Marchman VA (2007) Grammar and the Lexicon: developmental ordering in language acquisition. Child Dev 78:190–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Dove G (2012) Grammar as a developmental phenomenon. Biol Philos 27:615–637

    Google Scholar 

  • Dupré J (2014) The role of behavior in the recurrence of biological processes. Biol J Linn Soc 112:306–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Etxeberria A, Umerez J (2006) Organismo y organización en la biología teórica: ¿Vuelta al organicismo? Ludus Vitalis XIV:3–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT (2011) Unity and diversity of human language. Phil Trans R Soc B 366:376–388

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor J, Piattelli-Palmarini M (2010) What Darwin got wrong. Farrar Straus & Giroux, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gervain J, Mehler J (2010) Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annu Rev Pyschol 61:191–218

    Google Scholar 

  • Gervain J, Macagno F, Cogoi S, Pena M, Mehler J (2008) The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:14222–14227

    Google Scholar 

  • Gervain J, Berent I, Werker JF (2012) Binding at birth: the newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. J Cogn Neurosci 24:564–574

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert SF (2001) Ecological developmental biology: developmental biology meets the real world. Dev Biol 233:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert SF, Epel D (2009) Ecological developmental biology: integrating epigenetics, medicine, and evolution. Sinauer, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert SF, Epel D (2015) Ecological developmental biology: the environmental regulation of development, health, and evolution. Sinauer, Sunderland

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert SF, Opitz JM, Raff RA (1996) Resynthesizing evolutionary and developmental biology. Dev Biol 173:357–372

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi F, Bruni LE (2015) Developmental scaffolding. Biosemiotics 8:173–189

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman LR (1990) The structural sources of verb meanings. Lang Acquis 1:3–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman LR, Gillette J (1995) The role of syntax in verb learning. In: Fletcher P, MacWhinney B (eds) The handbook of child language. Wiley-Blackwell, Cambridge, pp 423–427

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold EM (1967) Language identification in the limit. Inf Control 10:447–474

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb G (1992) Individual development and evolution. The genesis of novel behavior. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb G (1997) Synthesizing nature-nurture: prenatal roots of instinctive behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb G (2007) Probabilistic epigenesis. Dev Sci 10:1–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield PM (1991) Language, tools and brain: the ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behav Brain Sci 14:531–595

    Google Scholar 

  • Griesemer JR (2014) Reproduction and the scaffolded development of hybrids. In: Caporael L, Griesemer JR, Wimsatt WC (eds) Developing scaffolds in evolution, culture, and cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 23–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths PE, Gray RD (1994) Developmental systems and evolutionary explanation. J Philos 91:277–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths PE, Gray RD (2001) Darwinism and developmental systems. In: Oyama S, Griffiths PE, Gray RD (eds) Cycles of contingency: developmental systems and evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 195–218

    Google Scholar 

  • Guasti MT (2017) Language acquisition.The growth of grammar, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Haeckel E (1877) Anthropogenie oder Entwickelungsgeschichte des Mensche. Wilhelm Engelman, Leipzig

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it? Who has it? How did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Yang C, Berwick RC, Tattersall I, Ryan MJ, Watumull J et al (2014) The mystery of language evolution. Front Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM (1999) The origins of grammar. Evidence from early language comprehension. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu S, Guasti MT, Gavarró A (2018) Chinese children’s knowledge of topicalization: experimental evidence from a comprehension study. J Psycholinguist Res 47:1279–1300

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyams N (1986) Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Reidel, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E, Lamb MJ (2005) Evolution in four dimensions. Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka E, Raz G (2009) Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: prevalence, mechanisms, and implications for the study of heredity and evolution. Q Rev Biol 84:131–175

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston TD (1987) The persistence of dichotomies in the study of behavioral development. Dev Rev 7:149–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston TD, Edwards L (2002) Genes, interactions, and the development of behavior. Psychol Rev 109:26–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston TD, Gottlieb G (1990) Neophenogenesis: a developmental theory of phenotypic evolution. J Theor Biol 147:471–495

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones M, Weismer SE, Evans JL, Lindstrom MJ (2007) Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. J Speech Lang Hear 50:508–528

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne R (1994) The antisymmetry of syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby S (2017) Culture and biology in the origins of linguistic structure. Psychon Bull Revi 24:118–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl PK (2007) Is speech learning ‘gated’ by the social brain? Dev Sci 10:110–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, O’Brien MJ (2011) Cultural niche construction: an introduction. Biol Theory 6:191–202

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Hoppitt W, Uller T (2013a) More on how and why: cause and effect in biology revisited. Biol Philos 28:719–745

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Hoppitt W, Uller T (2013b) More on how and why: a response to commentaries. Biol Philos 28:793–810

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Turner S (2014) The role of internal and external constructive processes in evolution. J Physiol 592:2413–2422

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Uller T, Feldman MW, Sterelny K, Müller GB, Moczek A et al (2015) The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proc R Soc B. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1019

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrman DS (1970) Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In: Aronson LR, Tobach E, Lehrman DS, Rosenblatt JS (eds) Development and evolution of behavior: essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla. Freeman, San Francisco, pp 17–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Leimar O, McNamara JM (2015) The evolution of transgenerational integration of information in heterogeneous environments. Am Nat 185:E55–E69

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin RC (1983) The organism as subject and object of evolution. Scientia, rivista internazionale de sintesi scientifica 118:65–82

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin RC (1998) The evolution of cognition: questions we will never answer. In: Scarborough D, Sternberg S (eds) An invitation to cognitive science. Methods, models, and conceptual issues, vol 4, 2nd edn. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 107–132

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin RC (2000) The triple helix. Gene, organism and environment. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke JL (1993) The child’s path to spoken language. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke JL (2006) Parental selection of vocal behavior: crying, cooing, babbling, and the evolution of language. Hum Nat 17:155–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke JL (2009) Evolutionary developmental linguistics: naturalization of the faculty of language. Lang Sci 31:33–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke JL, Bogin B (2006) Language and life history: a new perspective on the evolution and development of linguistic communication. Behav Brain Sci 29:259–325

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenzo G (2018) Long-distance paradox and the hybrid nature of language. Biosemiotics 11:387–404

    Google Scholar 

  • Love AC (2015) Conceptual change and evolutionary developmental biology. In: Love AC (ed) Conceptual change in biology. Scientific and philosophical perspectives on evolution and development. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 1–53

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney B (2008) Language evolution and human development. In: Ellis BJ, Bjorklund (eds) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford Press, New York, pp 383–410

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman V, Bates E (1994) Continuity in lexical and morphological development: a test of the critical mass hypothesis. J Child Lang 21:339–366

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman V, Thal D (2005) Words and grammar. In: Tomasello M, Slobin D (eds) Beyond nurture-nature: essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates. Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 141–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus GF, Vijayan S, Bandi RS, Vishton PM (1999) Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science 283:77–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr E (1982) The growth of biological thought. Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor K, Sheng L, Smith B (2005) The precocious two-year-old: status of the lexicon and links to the grammar. J Child Lang 32:563–585

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel GF, Moore CL (1995) Developmental psychobiology. An interdisciplinary science. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel GF, Tyler AN (2007) Developing human nature: ‘Development to’ versus ‘development from?’ Dev Psychobiol 49:788–799

    Google Scholar 

  • Minelli A (2003) The development of animal form. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Minelli A (2010) Evolutionary developmental biology does not offer a significant challenge to the Neo-Darwinian paradigm. In: Ayala FJ, Arp R (eds) Contemporary debates in philosophy of biology. Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester, pp 213–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Mivart St G (1871) On the genesis of species. Appleton, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Moczek AP (2008) On the origins of novelty in development and evolution. BioEssays 30:432–447

    Google Scholar 

  • Moczek AP, Sultan SE, Foster S, Ledón-Rettig C, Dworkin I, Nijhout HF et al (2011) The role of developmental plasticity in evolutionary innovation. Proc Royal Soc B 278:2705–2713

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore CL (2003) Evolution, development, and the individual acquisition of traits: what we’ve learned since Baldwin. In: Weber BH, Depew DJ (eds) Evolution and learning: the baldwin effect reconsidered. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 115–139

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro A (1995) Virtual conceptual necessity. La semplificazione della grammmatica nei primi anni novante. Ling Stile 30:637–674

    Google Scholar 

  • Neigles L (1990) Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. J Child Lang 17:357–374

    Google Scholar 

  • Newport EL (1990) Maturational constraints on language learning. Cogn Sci 14:11–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Notion P, Waring R (1997) Vocabulary size, text coverage and word lists. In: Schmitt N, McCarthy M (eds) Vocabulary: description, acquisition and pedagogy. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 6–19

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Griebel U (2008) Complexity and flexibility in infant vocal development and the earliest steps in the evolution of language. In: Oller DK, Griebel U (eds) Evolution of communicative flexibility: complexity, creativity and adaptability in human and animal communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 141–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Oller DK, Caskey M, Yoo H, Bene ER, Jhang Y, Lee C-C et al (2019) Preterm and full term infant vocalization and the origin of language. Sci Rep 9:14734. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-51352-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim RW (1981) Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behaviour: a neuroembriological perspective. In: Connolly KJ, Prechtl HFR (eds) Maturation and development: biological and psychological perspectives. Lippincott, Philadelphia, pp 73–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim RW (1984) Ontogenetic adaptations in neural and behavioural development: toward a more ‘ecological’ developmental psychobiology. In: Prechtl HFR (ed) Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life. Lippincott, Philadelphia, pp 16–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama S (2000a) The ontogeny of information. Developmental systems and evolution, 2nd edn. Duke University Press, Durham

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama S (2000b) Evolution’s eye. A systems view of the biology-culture divide. Duke University Press, Durham

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S (1991) Rules of language. Science 253:530–535

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S, Bloom P (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behav Brain Sci 13:707–784

    Google Scholar 

  • Pleyer M, Hartmann S (2019) Constructing a consensus in language evolution? Convergences and differences between biolinguistic and usage-based approaches. Front Psychol 10:2537. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02537

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Postal P (2003) (Virtually) conceptually necessary. J Linguist 39:599–620

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam H (1975) The meaning of “meaning”. Minn Stud Philos Sci 7:131–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Raff RA (1996) The shape of life. Genes, development, and the evolution of animal form. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid RGB (2007) Biological emergences. Evolution by natural experiment. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards RJ (1987) Darwin and the emergence of evolutionary theories of mind and behavior. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Robert JS (2004) Embryology, epigenesis, and evolution. Taking development seriously. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg A (1997) Reductionism redux: computing the embryo. Biol Philos 12:445–470

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi-Landi F (1968) Il linguaggio come lavoro e come mercato. Bompiani, Milano

    Google Scholar 

  • Skeide MA, Friederici AD (2016) The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:323–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf T, Corballis M (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30:299–313

    Google Scholar 

  • Sultan SE (2015) Organism and environment. Ecological development, niche construction, and adaptation. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sultan SE (2017a) Developmental plasticity: re-conceiving the genotype. Interface Focus. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2017.0009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sultan SE (2017b) Eco-Evo-Devo. In: Nuño de la Rosa L, Müller G (eds) Evolutionary developmental biology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33038-9_42-1

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sultan SE (2019) Genotype-environment interaction and the unscripted phenotype. In: Uller T, Laland KN (eds) Evolutionary causation. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 109–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Tattersall I (2019) The Minimalist Program and the origin of language: a view from paleoanthropology. Front Psychol 10:677. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00677

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2003) Constructing a language. A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2008) The origins of human communications. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Vercelli D, Piattelli-Palmarini M (2009) Language in an epigenetic framework. In: Piattelli-Palmarini M, Uriagereka J, Salaburu P (eds) Of minds and language. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 97–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky LS (2012) Language and thought. Revised and expanded edition. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner GP (2014) Homology, genes, and evolutionary innovation. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh DM (2015) Organisms, agency, and evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh DM (2019) The paradox of population thinking. In: Uller T, Laland K (eds) Cause and process in biology. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 227–246

    Google Scholar 

  • West MJ, King AP (1987) Settling nature and nurture into and ontogenetic niche. Dev Psychobiol 20:549–562

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wexler K (1990) Innateness and maturation in linguistic development. Dev Psychobiol 23:645–660

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt WC (1981) Robustness, reliability, and overdetermination. In: Brewer MB, Collins BE (eds) Scientific inquiry in the social sciences: a volume in honor of Donald T. Campbell. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp 123–162

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt WC (1986) Developmental constraints, generative entrenchment, and the innate-acquired distinction. In: Bechtel W (ed) Integrating scientific disciplines. Martinus Nijhoff, Dordrecht, pp 185–208

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt WC (1999) Generativity, entrenchment, evolution, and innateness: philosophy, evolutionary biology, and conceptual foundations of science. In: Harcastle V (ed) Where biology meets psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 137–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt WC (2001) Generative entrenchment and the developmental systems approach to evolutionary processes. In: Oyama S, Griffiths PE, Gray RD (eds) Cycles of contingency: developmental systems and evolution. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 219–237

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt WC (2015) Entrenchment as a theoretical tool in evolutionary developmental biology. In: Love AC (ed) Conceptual change in biology. Scientific and philosophical perspectives on evolution and development. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 365–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert L (1994) Do we understand development? Science 266:571–572

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This article has benefitted from a Grant from the Generalitat de Catalunya (2014-SGR-1013) to the Centre de Lingüística Teòrica (Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona) (SB) and a Grant from the Spanish Government (FFI2017-87699-P) (SB, GL and SES).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Guillermo Lorenzo.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Balari, S., Lorenzo, G. & Sultan, S.E. Language Acquisition and EcoDevo Processes: The Case of the Lexicon-Syntax Interface. Biol Theory 15, 148–160 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-020-00352-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752-020-00352-9

Keywords

Navigation