Skip to main content
Log in

Narrative Scaffolding

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Mental capacities, philosophers of mind and cognition have recently argued, are not exclusively realised in brain, but depend upon the rest of the body and the local environment. In this context, the concept of ‘scaffolding’ has been employed to specify the relationship between embodied organisms and their local environment. The core idea is that at least some cognitive and affective capacities are causally dependent upon environmental resources. However, in-depth examinations of specific examples of scaffolding as test cases for current theorising about scaffolding have remained scarce. The aim of the current paper is to help close this gap. To this end, I will offer a characterisation of key aspects of ‘scaffolding’ that can help specify scaffolding relations. In a second step, I will analyse fictional textual narrative as a test case for accounts of cognitive and affective scaffolding. The key claim of this paper will be that fictional textual narrative can be considered as a scaffold that transforms our capacities in social understanding and empathising in the course of ontogeny.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In what follows, ‘textual narrative’ refers to a class of material scaffolds. When talking about ‘a textual narrative’ or ‘textual narratives’, I refer to particular members of this class.

  2. It should be noted that the concept of ‘scaffolding’ was first introduced in psychological research on the role of tutoring in children’s acquisition of problem-solving skills (Wood et al. 1976). In this case, the scaffolding relation consists in the guidance provided by a parent, teacher or knowledgeable peer (the scaffold) and a problem-solving capacity (the scaffolded). In this paper, I will focus on philosophical work on scaffolding. For a review of educational research on ‘scaffolding’, see Pea (2004).

  3. Another option would be that cognitive and affective scaffolding would not influence each other in any relevant sense, given that cognitive and affective processes would be clearly distinct. However, I will not explore this modularist option further, given that it is implausible in light of empirical findings and theoretical considerations (see, e.g., Pessoa 2015; Todd et al. 2020).

  4. As stated above, I am concerned with the representation of connected events, not with the nature of connected events themselves. As Goldie (2012) points out, we should clearly distinguish between “a sequence of events; and a narrative or story of the events” (p. 7). Accordingly, the considerations developed in this sub-section are aesthetic and narratological, not ontological. For this reason, they are independent from discussions on the ontological properties of events and their connections. I am grateful to an anonymous reviewer for pressing me on this point.

  5. The notion of ‘transformation’ as it is used in this context is similar to Sterelny’s (2010) notion and refers to the ontogenetic development of cognitive capacities.

  6. An interesting test case for a transmedial narratological analysis of scaffolding relations would be audiobooks, which transpose the autoglottic space of a fictional textual narrative into a temporal sequence of vocalisations. Arguably, narrative discourse, story, and plot would be preserved in the transposition of a narrative text into an audiobook. However, we should expect to find differences between the identification and interpretation of visually detectable textual features and auditory cues (for a discussion, see Kuzmičová 2016). These differences, I assume, would influence the ways in which fictional textual and audiobook narratives could scaffold cognitive and affective capacities.

References

  • Acheson, D.J., J.B. Wells, and M.C. MacDonald. 2008. New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behavior Research Methods 40 (1): 278–289. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Pess.

  • Baron-Cohen, S., S. Wheelwright, J. Hill, Y. Raste, and I. Plumb. 2001. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 42 (2): 241–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, N.S. 2016. Words onscreen: The fate of reading in a digital world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Black, J.E., and J.L. Barnes. 2015. The effects of reading material on social and non-social cognition. Poetics 52: 32–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2015.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brockmeier, J. 2013. Fact and fiction: Exploring the narrative mind. In The travelling concepts of narrative, ed. M. Hatavara, L.-C. Hydén, and M. Hyvärinen, 121–140. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, M., and E.T. Troscianko, eds. 2017. Cognitive literary science: Dialogues between literature and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caracciolo, M. 2014. Tell-tale rhythms: Embodiment and narrative discourse. Storyworlds: A Journal of Narrative Studies, 6(2), 49–73.

  • Caracciolo, M., and T. Van Duuren 2015. Changed by literature? A critical review of psychological research on the effects of reading fiction. Interdisciplinary Literary Studies 17 (4): 517–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, N. 2010. On the narrative connection. In Beyond aesthetics: Philosophical essays, 118–133. Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511605970

  • Clark, A. 1997. Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. 2008. Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., and D. Chalmers. 1998. The extended mind. Analysis 58 (1): 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombetti, G., and J. Krueger. 2015. Scaffoldings of the affective mind. Philosophical Psychology 28 (8): 1157–1176. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2014.976334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coninx, S., and Stephan, A. 2021. A taxonomy of environmentally scaffolded affectivity. Danish Yearbook of Philosophy: 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1163/24689300-bja10019.

  • Currie, G. 2006. Narrative representation of causes. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 64 (3): 309–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, G. 2010. Narratives & narrators: A philosophy of stories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, G. 2016. Does fiction make us less empathic? Teorema: Revista Internacional de Filosofía, XXXV(3), 47–68.

  • de Vignemont, F., and P. Jacob 2012. What is it like to feel another’s pain? Philosophy of Science 79 (2): 295–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vignemont, F., and T. Singer 2006. The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 (10): 435–441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. 2010. Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demers, L.A., and N.S. Koven 2015. The relation of alexithymic traits to affective theory of mind. The American Journal of Psychology 128 (1): 31–42. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.128.1.0031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dodell-Feder, D., and D.I. Tamir 2018. Fiction reading has a small positive impact on social cognition: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 147 (11): 1713–1727. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabry, R.E. 2018a. Enculturation and narrative practices. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 17 (5): 911–937. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9567-x.

  • Fabry, R.E. 2018b. Betwixt and between: The enculturated predictive processing approach to cognition. Synthese 195 (6): 2483–2518. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1334-y.

  • Fludernik, M. 2014. Conversational narration - oral narration. In Handbook of narratology, ed. P. Hühn, J.C. Mester, J. Pier, and W. Schmid, 93–104. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Freiman, M. 2015. The art of drafting and revision: Extended mind in creative writing. New Writing 12 (1): 48–66. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790726.2014.977797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2012. Empathy, simulation, and narrative. Science in Context 25 (3): 355–381. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0269889712000117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2020. Action and interaction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., and D.D. Hutto 2008. Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, 17–38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goldie, P. 2009. Narrative thinking, emotion, and planning. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 67 (1): 97–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldie, P. 2012. The mess inside: Narrative, emotion, and the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A.I. 2006. Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mindreading. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. 1997. Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Pess.

  • Griffiths, P., and A. Scarantino 2009. Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotions. In The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition, ed. P. Robbins and M. Aydede, 437–453. Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harbus, A. 2011. Exposure to life-writing as an impact on autobiographical memory. Memory Studies 4 (2): 206–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698010389571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R. 1989. How does writing restructure thought? Language & Communication 9 (2–3): 99–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. 2020. The weirdest people in the world: How the West became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman, D. 2017. Storytelling and the sciences of the mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. 2018. Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. 1954. The crisis of European sciences and transcendental phenomenology. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D.D. 2007. The narrative practice hypothesis: Origins and applications of folk psychology. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplement 60: 43–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D.D. 2008. The narrative practice hypothesis: Clarifications and implications. Philosophical Explorations 11 (3): 175–192. https://doi.org/10.1080/13869790802245679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. 1990. Technology and the lifeworld: From garden to earth. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, D.C., and E. Castano 2013. Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science 342 (6156): 377–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, D., and E. Castano. 2019. Reading literary fiction and theory of mind: Three preregistered replications and extensions of Kidd and Castano (2013). Social Psychological and Personality Science 10 (4): 522–531. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550618775410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J. 2020. Schizophrenia and the scaffolded self. Topoi 39 (3): 597–609. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-018-9547-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kukkonen, K. 2014. Plot. In Handbook of narratology, 2nd ed., ed. P. Hühn, J.C. Mester, J. Pier, and W. Schmid, 706–719. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kukkonen, K. 2015. Space, time, and causality in graphic narratives: An embodied approach. In From comic strips to graphic novels: Contributions to the theory and history of graphic narrative, 2nd ed., ed. D. Stein and J.-N. Thon, 49–66. Berlin, Boston: Walter de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kukkonen, K. 2018. A moving target–cognitive narratology and feminism. Textual Practice 32 (6): 973–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kukkonen, K. (2019). 4e cognition and eighteenth century-fiction: How the novel found its feet. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Kukkonen, K. 2020. Probability designs: Literature and predictive processing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kukkonen, K., and H.S. Nielsen 2018. Fictionality: Cognition and exceptionality. Poetics Today 39 (3): 473–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuzmičová, A. 2016. Audiobooks and print narrative: Similarities in text experience. In Audionarratology: Interfaces of Sound and Narrative, ed. J. Mildorf and T. Kinzel, 217–237. Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuzmičová, A., and K. Bálint. 2019. Personal relevance in story reading: A research review. Poetics Today 40 (3): 429–451. https://doi.org/10.1215/03335372-7558066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamarque, P. 2004. On not expecting too much from narrative. Mind & Language 19 (4): 393–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamarque, P. 2007. On the distance between literary narratives and real-life narratives. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements 60: 117–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamarque, P. 2016. Peter Goldie on narrative thinking. In Art, mind, and narrative: Themes from the work of Peter Goldie, ed. J. Dodd, 47–60. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Langkau, J. 2020. The empathic skill fiction can’t teach us. Philosophical Psychology 33 (3): 313–331. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2020.1731446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maiese, M. 2016. Affective scaffolds, expressive arts, and cognition. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mar, R.A., and K. Oatley 2008. The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3 (3): 173–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2007. Writing as thinking. Language Sciences 29 (5): 621–632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2007.01.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2010a. Dimensions of mind. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9 (4): 561–578. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9186-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R., ed. 2010b. The extended mind. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2014. Neural plasticity, neuronal recycling and niche construction. Mind & Language 29 (3): 286–303. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menary, R. 2015. Mathematical cognition: A case of enculturation. In Open MIND, ed. T. Metzinger and J. M. Windt, 1–20. Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570818

  • Michael, J. 2014. Towards a consensus about the role of empathy in interpersonal understanding. Topoi 33 (1): 157–172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-013-9204-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mittell, J. 2007. Film and television narrative. In The Cambridge companion to narrative, ed. D. Herman, 156–171. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mumper, M.L., and R.J. Gerrig 2019. How does leisure reading affect social cognitive abilities? Poetics Today 40 (3): 453–473. https://doi.org/10.1215/03335372-7558080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newen, A. (2015). Understanding others: The person model theory. In Open MIND, ed. T. Metzinger and J. M. Windt, 1–28. Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570320

  • Newen, A., L. de Bruin, and S. Gallagher, eds. 2018. Oxford handbook of 4e cognition: Embodied, enacted, extended and embedded. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D.R. 2016. The mind on paper: Reading, consciousness and rationality. Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ong, W.J. 2012. Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. London, New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panero, M.E., D.S. Weisberg, J. Black, T.R. Goldstein, J.L. Barnes, H. Brownell, and E. Winner. 2016. Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 111 (5): e46–e54. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pea, R.D. 2004. The social and technological dimensions of scaffolding and related theoretical concepts for learning, education, and human activity. Journal of the Learning Sciences 13 (3): 423–451. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327809jls1303_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. 2015. Précis on the cognitive-emotional brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 38: 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polvinen, M. 2013. Affect and artifice in cognitive literary theory. Journal of Literary Semantics 42 (2): 165–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polvinen, M. 2017. Cognitive science and the double vision of fiction. In Cognitive literary science: Dialogues between literature and cognition, ed. M. Burke and E.T. Troscianko, 135–150. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Polvinen, M. (2018). A simple story of a complex mind? In Narrating complexity, ed. R. Walsh and S. Stepney, 65–79. Cham: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-64714-2_7

  • Ramsey, W.M. 2007. Representation reconsidered. Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., B.J. Juhasz, and A. Pollatsek 2007. Eye movements during reading. In The science of reading: A handbook, ed. M.J. Snowling and C. Hulme, 79–97. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. 2007. Deeper than reason: Emotion and its role in literature, music, and art. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M.-L. 2007. Toward a definition of narrative. In The Cambridge companion to narrative, ed. D. Herman, 22–35. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saarinen, J.A. 2019. Paintings as solid affective scaffolds. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 77 (1): 67–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saarinen, J.A. 2020. What can the concept of affective scaffolding do for us? Philosophical Psychology 33 (6): 820–839. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2020.1761542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samur, D., M. Tops, and S.L. Koole. 2018. Does a single session of reading literary fiction prime enhanced mentalising performance? Four replication experiments of Kidd and Castano (2013). Cognition and Emotion 32 (1): 130–144. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1279591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaeffer, J.-M. 2013. Fictional vs. factual narration. In The living handbook of narratology. Hamburg University. Retrieved from http://www.lhn.uni-hamburg.de/article/fictional-vs-factual-narration

  • Smortchkova, J., K. Dołęga, and T. Schlicht, eds. 2020. What are mental representations? Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, A., S. Walter, and W. Wilutzky. 2014. Emotions beyond brain and body. Philosophical Psychology 27 (1): 65–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2013.828376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. 2010. Minds: Extended or scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9 (4): 465–481. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9174-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. 2016. Scaffolding memory: Themes, taxonomies, puzzles. In Contextualizing human memory: An interdisciplinary approach to understanding how individuals and groups remember the past, ed. L. Bietti and C.B. Stone, 187–205. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, R.M., V. Miskovic, J. Chikazoe, and A.K. Anderson 2020. Emotional objectivity: Neural representations of emotions and their interaction with cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 71 (1): 25–48. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-051044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varga, S. 2019. Scaffolded minds: Integration and disintegration. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Velleman, J.D. 2003. Narrative explanation. The Philosophical Review 112 (1): 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D., J.S. Bruner, and G. Ross 1976. The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 17 (2): 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am greatly indebted to Roy Dings and Jussi Saarinen for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. I would also like to thank Sabrina Coninx, Maja Griem, Albert Newen, Alfredo Vernazzani, and Julia Wolf for their constructive feedback. My work on large parts of the paper was made possible by a postdoctoral position, funded by Ruhr University Bochum, within the Research Training Group Situated Cognition. I would like to thank the group members for their support. The revision of this paper was funded by an ARC Discovery Early Career Research Award (grant number DE210100115).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Regina E. Fabry.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fabry, R.E. Narrative Scaffolding. Rev.Phil.Psych. 14, 1147–1167 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00595-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00595-w

Keywords

Navigation