Skip to main content
Log in

Rewarding one’s Future Self: Psychological Connectedness, Episodic Prospection, and a Puzzle about Perspective

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

When faced with intertemporal choices, which have consequences that unfold over time, we often discount the future, preferring smaller immediate rewards often at the expense of long-term benefits. How psychologically connected one feels to one’s future self-influences such temporal discounting. Psychological connectedness consists in sharing psychological properties with past or future selves, but connectedness comes in degrees. If one feels that one is not psychologically connected to one’s future self, one views that self like a different person and is less likely to wait for the future reward. Increasing perceived psychological connectedness to one’s future self may lead to more far-sighted decisions. Episodic prospection may help in this regard. Episodic prospection is our ability to ‘pre-experience’ the future by mentally simulating it, drawing on information from episodic memory and other sources. Episodic memory and prospection are thought to involve a special form of consciousness, which underpins the capacity to appreciate the connection between one’s past, present, and future selves. Simulating the future self through prospection may increase felt psychological connectedness and support future-oriented decision-making. Yet this is where a puzzle arises. The imagery of episodic memory and prospection is perspectival: often one views the visualised scenario from a detached perspective, seeing oneself from-the-outside as if viewing another person. The aim of this paper is to characterise how the perspectival imagery of prospection relates to psychological connectedness, and to show that even though such imagery involves a detached perspective it can still be used to help reward one’s future self.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For a discussion of how perspectival imagery in mental time travel relates to emotion and discounting the future, see McCarroll (2019).

  2. From a normative point of view, the optimal way to choose between smaller immediate and larger later rewards means that preferences should be consistent across time, and would be expressed by an exponential discount function. Yet temporal discounting across domains typically describes a hyperbola-like function, with the discount rate declining with time, rather than the exponential function with a constant discount rate (Meyerson and Green, 1995). In fact, one way to measure the discounting of delayed rewards is to calculate the area under the curve (Meyerson, Green, and Warusawitharana, 2001). Our aim in this paper is not to take a stance on the function best described by temporal discounting, nor to explain the reasons why we choose inconsistently across time, but to look at one possible mechanism that may attenuate temporal discounting however it occurs.

  3. For differences and similarities between the ‘temporal discounting’ and the ‘delay of gratification’ paradigms, see Reynolds and Schiffbauer (2005).

  4. We are open to the possibility that not all failures to delay gratification involve such a trade-off between subsequent selves – at least, not at a conscious level. In this paper we are referring to cases which do seem to involve a trade-off between a present and future self.

  5. The classic experiments involving young children waiting (or not) to be rewarded with marshmallows, by Mischel and colleagues, showed that delaying gratification and resisting the temptation of the smaller immediate reward was correlated with better scores in school and less problems with drugs and alcohol in later life (e.g., Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989, but see Watts, Duncan, & Quan, 2018).

  6. Interestingly, the first study to test the relation between psychological connectedness and temporal discounting (Frederick, 2003) found no correlation between connectedness (operationalised as ‘similarity’) and discount rates. Frederick himself offers two explanations as to why no correlation was found in this study. He suggests that ‘it might indicate that people (implicitly) endorse the simple view of personal identity – that they believe that they are the same person through time, and that changes in personality (or “connectedness”) is just not one of the things that should affect their valuation of future rewards’ (Frederick, 2003, p. 99). Or it could simply be that the type of ‘matching task’ employed in the study, whereby respondents were asked to report the amount of money they would require in 1/5/10/20/30/40 years to make them indifferent to receiving $100 tomorrow, may be too abstract and ‘reflect idiosyncratic algorithms for “solving the task” as much as they reflect anything about time preference per se’ (Frederick, 2003, p. 99).

  7. See, for example, Frederick (2003); Ersner-Hershfield et al. (2009); Ersner-Hershfield, Wimmer, and Knutson (2009); Bartels and Rips (2010); Bartels and Urminsky (2011). See Urminsky 2017 for a nice summary.

  8. In the psychological literature investigating the relation between psychological connectedness and intertemporal choice, the tendency to not wait for the delayed reward, especially one in the farther future as opposed to the near future, is often termed ‘impatience’, to be contrasted with ‘patience’, which means that one is more willing to wait for the larger later reward. Arguably, these terms are imprecise, but we employ them here because they are used in the literature we draw on.

  9. Bartels and Urminsky’s (2011) series of five studies distinguishes connectedness from other factors associated with time preference. They provide evidence that the impact of connectedness on long-term discount rates cannot be explained by factors such as change in general life circumstances, uncertainty about the future and one’s future preferences, present-bias (the general tendency to be more impatient for outcomes in the immediate future than for distant future outcomes), or differences in the affective appraisal of future outcomes. They also found that, at least in one study, the effects of manipulating connectedness on discounting generalised to a demographically broad adult population; in particular, they found no reduction in the effect of the manipulation on older participants.

  10. In some cases, for example in individuals with episodic amnesia, this capacity is impaired (for discussion see below). Further, there is debate in the literature whether nonhuman animals have the capacity to mentally travel in time. As our focus in this paper is solely on human subjects this is not a debate we enter into here, but see, e.g., Clayton et al. (2003); Suddendorf and Corballis (2007).

  11. Only two studies to date have directly tested this hypothesis on individuals with episodic amnesia and the evidence is mixed, with one study failing to find any attenuation of temporal discounting (Palombo et al., 2015) and another finding attenuation of temporal discounted (Kwan et al., 2015) when individuals with episodic amnesia were cued to engage in episodic prospection.

  12. Hoerl and McCormack also think it is plausible that episodic prospection might reduce the subjective temporal distance of a future event, which is a further factor that has been implicated in temporal discounting (2016, p. 243).

  13. See Rice (2010) for an excellent review of the empirical evidence on field and observer perspectives in episodic memory.

  14. Nichols is discussing evidence by Klein et al., (2002), and Tulving (1993), and provides the examples of patients D.B. and K.C., who ascribed their character traits consistently across time, and consistent with ratings from family members. An intriguing question is whether it could be that the spared performance of amnesiac individuals on intertemporal choice tasks, whose discounting rates can even be modified by cues prompting them to ‘imagine specific personal future events temporally contiguous with the receipt of delayed reward’ (Kwan et al., 2015, p. 432), is partly underpinned by stored semantic trait knowledge which makes them feel psychologically connected to their future selves.

  15. Our focus in this paper has been on the relation between observer perspectives in episodic prospection and psychological connectedness. However, we do not want to suggest that field perspectives are not relevant to overcoming temporal discounting or delaying gratification. One possibility is that the differing visual perspectives may support future-oriented decision-making at different temporal distances. Because field perspectives tend to focus more on subjective details, they may make rewards in the near future more salient, increasing feelings of continuity in a phenomenological sense (autonoesis) (Prebble et al., 2013). Because observer perspectives involve a more abstract way of thinking, such images may make an event in the farther future more salient, promoting continuity on a conceptual level (psychological connectedness). In fact, there is evidence that remembering or imagining an event in one’s life may involve both perspectives (Rice and Rubin, 2009). Perhaps this multiperspectival imagery blends both facets of the self travelling in subjective time—autonoesis and psychological connectedness—focusing on a mix of salient information that may be relevant for an intertemporal choice.

References

  • Ainslie, G. 1975. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin 82: 463–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. 1988/2008. Thinking and deciding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, D.M., and L.J. Rips. 2010. Psychological connectedness and Intertemporal choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 139 (1): 49–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels, D.M., and O. Urminsky. 2011. On Intertemporal selfishness: How the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. Journal of Consumer Research 38: 182–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benoit, R.G., S.J. Gilbert, and P.W. Burgess. 2011. A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (18): 6771–6779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berns, G.S., D. Laibson, and G. Loewenstein. 2007. Intertemporal choice―toward an integrative framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 (11): 482–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D., and A. Bohn. 2010. Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory and Cognition 38: 265–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. 2008. Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 219–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clayton, N.S., T.J. Bussey, and A. Dickinson. 2003. Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 685–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosentino, E. 2011. Self in time and language. Consciousness and Cognition 20: 777–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosentino, E., and F. Ferretti. 2015. Cognitive foundations of the narrative self. Rivista Internazionale di Filosofia e Psicologia 6 (2): 311–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, T.O., C.M. Stanton, and L.H. Epstein. 2013. The future is now: Comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite 71: 120–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, T.O., M. Said, C.M. Stanton, and L.H. Epstein. 2015. Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and energy intake in children. Eating Behaviors 18: 20–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A. 2016. The role of personal goals in future-oriented mental time travel. In Seeing the future: Theoretical perspectives on future-oriented mental time travel, ed. K. Michaelian, S.B. Klein, and K.K. Szpunar, 199–214. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A., and M. Van der Linden. 2012. Predicting the phenomenology of episodic future thoughts. Consciousness and Cognition 21: 1198–1206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., M.T. Garton, K. Ballard, G.R. Samanez-Larkin, and B. Knutson. 2009. Don’t stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving. Judgment and Decision making 4 (4): 280–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., G.E. Wimmer, and B. Knutson. 2009. Saving for the future self: Neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. Scan 4: 85–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, J. 2018. Observer memory and immunity to error through misidentification. Synthese: 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-018-02050-3.

  • Finnbogadóttir, H., and D. Berntsen. 2014. Looking at life from different angles: Observer perspective during remembering and imagining distinct emotional events. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 1 (4): 387–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. G., & Gilovich, T. (1989). Effect of Memory Perspective on Retrospective Causal Attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(3), 399-403.

  • Frederick, S. 2003. Time preference and personal identity. In Time and decision, ed. G. Loewenstein, D. Read, and R. Baumeister, 89–113. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Y. Trope, N. Liberman, and M. Levin-Sagi. 2006. Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 351–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., and J.J. Carnevale. 2012. Transcending temptation through abstraction: The role of construal level in self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science 21 (4): 248–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D.T., and T.D. Wilson. 2007. Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317 (5843): 1351–1354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grilli, M.D., and M. Verfaellie. 2015. Supporting the self-concept with memory: Insight from amnesia. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10 (12): 1684–1692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hershfield, H.E., D.G. Goldstein, W.F. Sharpe, J. Fox, L. Yeykelis, L.L. Carstensen, and J.N. Bailenson. 2011. Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. Journal of Marketing Research 48: S23–S37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoerl, C., and T. McCormack. 2016. Making decisions about the future: Regret and the cognitive function of episodic memory. In Seeing the future: Theoretical perspectives on future-oriented mental time travel, ed. K. Michaelian, S.B. Klein, and K.K. Szpunar, 241–266. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Irish, M. 2016. Semantic memory as the essential scaffold for future-oriented mental time travel. In Seeing the future: Theoretical perspectives on future-oriented mental time travel, ed. K. Michaelian, S.B. Klein, and K.K. Szpunar, 389–408. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971/1972). The Actor and the Observer: Divergent Perceptions of the Causes of Behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior (pp. 79-94). Morristown: General Learning Press.

  • Klein, S.B. 2016. Autonoetic consciousness: Reconsidering the role of episodic memory in future-oriented self-projection. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (2): 381–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, S.B., K. Rozendale, and L. Cosmides. 2002. A social-cognitive neuroscience analysis of the self. Social Cognition 20: 105–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, D., C.F. Craver, L. Green, J. Myerson, P. Boyer, and R.S. Rosenbaum. 2012. Future decision-making without episodic mental time travel. Hippocampus 22: 1215–1219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, D., C.F. Craver, L. Green, J. Myerson, F. Gao, S.E. Black, and R.S. Rosenbaum. 2015. Cueing the personal future to reduce discounting in Intertemporal choice: Is episodic prospection necessary? Hippocampus 25 (4): 432–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libby, L.K., and R.P. Eibach. 2011. Visual perspective in mental imagery: A representational tool that functions in judgment, emotion, and self-insight. In Advances in experimental social psychology, ed. M.P. Zanna and J.M. Olson, vol. 44, 185–245. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libby, L.K., and R.P. Eibach. 2013. The role of visual imagery in social cognition. In The Oxford handbook of social cognition, ed. D.E. Carlston, 147–166. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libby, L. K., & Eibach, R. P. (2002). Looking back in time: Self-concept change affects visual perspective in autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(2), 167–179.

  • Libby, L.K., E.M. Shaeffer, R.P. Eibach, and J.A. Slemmer. 2007. Picture yourself at the polls: Visual perspective in mental imagery affects self-perception and behavior. Psychological Science 18: 199–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthen, M. 2010. Is memory preservation? Philosophical Studies 148 (1): 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarroll, C.J. 2018. Remembering from the outside: Personal memory and the perspectival mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McCarroll, C.J. 2019. Navigating Intertemporal choices: Mental time travel, perspectival imagery, and prudent decision-making. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 6 (2): 200–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarroll, C.J., and J. Sutton. 2017. Memory and perspective. In The Routledge handbook of philosophy of memory, ed. S. Bernecker and K. Michaelian, 113–126. London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K.B., C.L. Wooldridge, H.J. Rice, J.J. Berg, and K.K. Szpunar. 2016. Visual perspective in remembering and episodic future thought. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (2): 243–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyerson, J., and L. Green. 1995. Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 64 (3): 263–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyerson, J., L. Green, and M. Warusawitharana. 2001. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 76 (2): 235–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michaelian, K. 2016. Mental time travel: Episodic memory and our knowledge of the personal past. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Y. Shoda, and M.I. Rodriguez. 1989. Delay of gratification in children. Science 244: 933–938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, B.S., D.R. Sherrod, T.J. Liu, and B. Underwood. 1979. The dispositional shift in attribution over time. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 15: 553–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nanay, B. 2016. The role of imagination in decision-making. Mind and Language 31 (1): 127–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. 2017. Memory and personal identity. In The Routledge handbook of philosophy of memory, ed. S. Bernecker and K. Michaelian, 169–179. London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nigro, G., and U. Neisser. 1983. Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology 15: 467–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., Caputo, C., Legant, P., & Marecek, J. (1973). Behavior as Seen by the Actor and as Seen by the Observer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(2), 154-164.

  • Nussbaum, S., Y. Trope, and N. Liberman. 2003. Creeping dispositionism: The temporal dynamics of behavior prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84: 485–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paglieri, F. 2012. Ulysses’ will: Self-control, external constraints, and games. In Consciousness in interaction: The role of the natural and social context in shaping consciousness, ed. F. Paglieri, 179–206. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Palombo, D.J., M.M. Keane, and M. Verfaellie. 2015. The medial temporal lobes are critical for reward-based decision making under conditions that promote episodic future thinking. Hippocampus 25: 345–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parfit, D. 1984. Reasons and persons. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrin, D. 2016. Asymmetries in subjective time. In Seeing the future: Theoretical perspectives on future-oriented mental time travel, ed. K. Michaelian, S.B. Klein, and K.K. Szpunar, 39–61. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., and C. Büchel. 2010. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-Mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66: 138–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., and C. Büchel. 2011. The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: Understanding variability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15 (5): 227–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prebble, S.C., D.R. Addis, and L.J. Tippett. 2013. Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychological Bulletin 139 (4): 815–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., C.Y. Olivola, and K.A. Kennedy. 2008. Doing unto future selves as you would do unto others: Psychological distance and decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 (2): 224–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., and L. Ross. 2006. Temporal differences in trait self ascription: When the self is seen as an other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90 (2): 197–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quoidbach, J., D.T. Gilbert, and T.D. Wilson. 2013. The end of history illusion. Science 339 (6115): 96–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D.D., and J.K. Luiselli. 2011. Temporal discounting. In Encyclopedia of child behavior and development, ed. S. Goldstein and J.A. Naglieri, 1474–1474. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, B., and R. Schiffbauer. 2005. Delay of gratification and delay discounting: A unifying feedback model of delay-related impulsive behavior. The Psychological Record 55: 439–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, H.J. 2010. Seeing where We’re at: A review of visual perspective and memory retrieval. In The act of remembering: Toward an understanding of how we recall the past, ed. J.H. Mace, 228–258. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, H.J., and D.C. Rubin. 2009. I can see it both ways: First- and third-person visual perspectives at retrieval. Consciousness and Cognition 18: 877–890.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., and M.C. Corballis. 2007. The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J. 2010. Observer perspective and Acentred memory: Some puzzles about point of view in personal memory. Philosophical Studies 148 (1): 27–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K.K., and E. Tulving. 2011. Varieties of future experience. In Predictions in the brain: Using our past to generate a future, ed. M. Bar, 3–12. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., and N. Liberman. 2003. Temporal Construal. Psychological Review 110 (3): 403–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., and N. Liberman. 2010. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review 117 (2): 440–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. 1985. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology 26 (1): 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. 1993. Self-knowledge of an amnesic individual is represented abstractly. In Advances in social cognition, ed. T.K. Srull and R.S. Wyer, vol. 5, 1–49. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. 2002. Chronesthesia: Conscious awareness of subjective time. In Principles of frontal lobe function, ed. D.T. Stuss and R.T. Knight. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urminsky, O. 2017. The role of psychological connectedness to the future self in decisions over time. Current Directions in Psychological Science 26 (1): 34–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urminsky, O., and D.M. Bartels. 2011. Looking into Future’s Mirror: How representations of the aged self impact impatience. In NA - advances in consumer research, ed. R. Ahluwalia, T.L. Chartrand, and R.K. Ratner, vol. 39, 278. Duluth: Association for Consumer Research.

  • Urminsky, O., and G. Zauberman. 2015. The psychology of Intertemporal preferences. In The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making, ed. G. Keren and G. Wu, vol. I, 141–181. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, T.W., G.J. Duncan, and H. Quan. 2018. Revisiting the marshmallow test: A conceptual replication investigating links between early delay of gratification and later outcomes. Psychological Science 29: 1159–1177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This paper is the result of work done on the project ‘The cornerstone of self-control: A philosophical investigation into the role of mental time travel in overcoming temporal discounting.’ We gratefully acknowledge the funding for this project, which was a grant provided by the Templeton Foundation through Florida State University’s project ‘The Philosophy and Science of Self-Control’. This work is also supported by the French National Research Agency in the framework of the “Investissements d’avenir” program (ANR-15-IDEX-02). Versions of this paper were presented at the workshop ‘Memory, Mental Time Travel, and Self-Control’, at Roma Tre University; the workshop ‘Whole Lives, Time, and Selfhood’, at Deakin University; and the Australasian Association of Philosophy conference at Victoria University, Wellington. The authors would like to thank the participants at those events for helpful comments and discussion, especially Dorothea Debus, Francesco Ferretti, Natalie Gold, Richard Heersmink, Christoph Hoerl, Teresa McCormack, Kirk Michaelian, Fabio Paglieri, Marya Schechtman, Kim Sterelny, Patrick Stokes, John Sutton, Dan Weijers, and Markus Werning. We would also like to thank Bence Nanay, Gerardo Viera, Kevin Lande, Loraine Gérardin-Laverge, Constant Bonard, Alma Barner, Peter Fazekas, Magdalini Koukou, Denis Perrin, André Sant’Anna, Reza Mosmer, Alberto Guerrero-Velázquez, Paloma Muñoz, and two anonymous reviewers for very helpful comments on previous drafts.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher Jude McCarroll.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McCarroll, C.J., Cosentino, E. Rewarding one’s Future Self: Psychological Connectedness, Episodic Prospection, and a Puzzle about Perspective. Rev.Phil.Psych. 11, 449–467 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-020-00460-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-020-00460-2

Keywords

Navigation