Skip to main content
Log in

Explaining the Illusion of Asymmetric Insight

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

People tend to think that they know others better than others know them (Pronin et al. in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 639–656, 2001). This phenomenon is known as the “illusion of asymmetric insight.” While the illusion has been well documented by a series of recent experiments, less has been done to explain it. In this paper, we argue that extant explanations are inadequate because they either get the explanatory direction wrong or fail to accommodate the experimental results in a sufficiently nuanced way. Instead, we propose a new explanation that does not face these problems. The explanation is based on two other well-documented psychological phenomena: the tendency to accommodate ambiguous evidence in a biased way, and the tendency to overestimate how much better we know ourselves than we know others.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See Schlenker and Miller (1977), Greenwald (1980), Riess et al. (1981), Greenberg et al. (1982), and Mezulis et al. (2004) for studies and reviews of the self-enhancement bias.

  2. See Ichheiser (1949), Ross and Ward (1996), and Gilovich and Ross (2015) for studies and reviews of naïve realism.

  3. See also Goffman (1959), Markus (1983), and Andersen and Ross (1984) for studies and discussions of people’s convictions about their own and other people’s knowability.

  4. On another possible reading of Pronin et al.’s discussion of the introspection illusions, they do no seek to explain the illusion of asymmetric insight in terms of the introspection illusion, but rather seek to explain both of these illusions in terms of the knowability illusion. However, even if this is the correct interpretation, our new explanation still provides a unified account of all three illusions, by providing an independent explanation of the illusion of asymmetric insight, which in turn explains the other two.

  5. For useful overviews, see Gilovich (1991, ch. 3) and Oswald and Grosjean (2004).

  6. See Vazire (2010) for further discussion of motivational limitations to self-knowledge.

  7. See, e.g., Nisbett and Wilson (1977) for a classic study.

  8. See e.g. Lun et al. (2008) and Oishi et al. (2013) for recent studies and literature reviews on people’s perception of being misunderstood by others, and the importance of this for their well-being and feeling of being socially connected.

  9. These effects are illustrated in Figure 1 (Pronin et al. 2001: 644).

  10. We are grateful to an anonymous reviewer for bringing this effect to our attention.

  11. We thank an anonymous reviewer for urging us to consider this.

References

  • Andersen, S.M., and L. Ross. 1984. Self knowledge and social inference: I. The impact of cognitive/affective and behavioral data. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46: 280–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. 2008. Cartesian epistemology: Is the theory of the self-transparent mind innate? Journal of Consciousness Studies 15 (4): 28–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. 2010. Introspection: Divided and partly eliminated. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 80: 76–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T. 1991. How we know what isn’t so. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilovich, T., and L. Ross. 2015. The wisest one in the room: How you can benefit from social psychology’s most powerful insights. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. 1959. The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. 1993. How we know our minds: The illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1): 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., T. Pyszczynski, and S. Sheldon. 1982. The self-serving attributional Bias: Beyond self-presentation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 18 (1): 56–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. 1980. The totalitarian ego. American Psychologist 35: 603–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ichheiser, G. 1949. Misunderstandings in human relations: A study in false perception. American Journal of Sociology 55 (Suppl): 1–70.

  • Jones, E., and R. Nisbett. 1972. The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the cause of behavior. In Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior, ed. E. Jones, D. Kanouse, H. Kelley, R. Nisbett, S. Valins, and B. Weiner, 79–94. New York: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, K., and E. Pronin. 2008. When disagreement gets ugly: Perceptions of Bias and the escalation of conflict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 (6): 833–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozuch, B., and S. Nichols. 2011. Awareness of unawareness. Folk psychology and introspective transparency. Journal of Consciousness Studies 18 (11–12): 135–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunda, Z. 1990. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 108: 480–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., L. Ross, and M. Lepper. 1979. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 2098–2109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lun, J., S. Kesebir, and S. Oishi. 2008. On feeling understood and feeling well: The role of interdependence. Journal of Personality 42 (6): 1623–1628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. 1983. Self-knowledge: An expanded view. Journal of Personality 51: 543–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mezulis, A., L. Abramson, J. Hyde, and B. Hankin. 2004. Is there a universal positivity Bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin 130 (5): 711–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R., and T. Wilson. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84: 231–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., S. Akimoto, J. Richards, and E. Suh. 2013. Feeling understood as a key to cultural differences in life satisfaction. Journal of Research in Personality 47 (5): 488–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, M., and S. Grosjean. 2004. Confirmation bias. In Cognitive illusions. A Handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgment and memory, ed. R. Pohl, 79–98. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E. 2009. The introspection illusion. In Advances in experimental social psychology, ed. M. Zanna, vol. 41, 1–67. Cambridge: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., and M. Kugler. 2007. Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (4): 565–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., J. Kurger, K. Savitsky, and L. Ross. 2001. You don’t know me, but I know you: The illusion of asymmetric insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 639–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., D. Lin, and L. Ross. 2002. The Bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 (3): 369–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., T. Gilovich, and L. Ross. 2004. Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Divergent perceptions of bias in self versus others. Psychological Review 111 (3): 781–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., J. Fleming, and M. Steffel. 2008. Value revelations: Disclosure is in the eye of the beholder. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96: 795–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riess, M., P. Rosenfeld, V. Melburg, and J. Tedeschi. 1981. Self-serving attributions: Biased private perceptions and distorted public descriptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41 (2): 224–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, L., and A. Ward. 1996. Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In Values and knowledge, ed. T. Brown, E. Reed, and E. Turiel, 103–135. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, B., and R. Miller. 1977. Egocentrism in groups: Self-serving biases or logical information processing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35 (10): 755–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vazire, S. 2010. Who knows what about a person? The self-other knowledge asymmetry (SOKA) model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 98: 281–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. 1960. On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 14: 129–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T., and E. Dunn. 2003. Self-knowledge: Its limits, value, and potential for improvement. Annual Review of Psychology 55: 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Asbjørn Steglich-Petersen.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Steglich-Petersen, A., Skipper, M. Explaining the Illusion of Asymmetric Insight. Rev.Phil.Psych. 10, 769–786 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-019-00435-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-019-00435-y

Keywords

Navigation