Introduction

Parent–child book reading is one of the most important investments that parents make in their children’s language, literacy and lifelong learning (Kalb and van Ours 2014; Mistry et al. 2010; Thomas 2006). Regular reading to children and access to books is an essential component of a stimulating home learning environment (Powell and Diamond 2012). Shared book reading promotes oral language and provides developmental bridging from oral language to literacy (Senechal and LeFevre 2002). A recent study using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) identified a neurobiological correlate of parent–child book reading. When listening to stories, children with greater exposure to home reading showed higher activation of brain regions associated with language and literacy (Hutton et al. 2015).

A consistent finding in the literature is that regular parent–child book reading lessens the risk for low school readiness associated with socio-economic disadvantage and the cluster of psychosocial risk exposures associated with it (Duncan et al. 2007; Lugo-Gil and Tamis-LeMonda 2008; Mistry et al. 2010; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2015). This is encouraging for early childhood professionals, whose sphere of influence centres on enhancing parent–child interactions and home learning experiences (Guralnick 2013; Knoche et al. 2012).

Promoting parent–child book reading is an uncontested essential role for early childhood professionals and is championed in early childhood education (United Nations Educational Scientific and Cultural Organisation, UNESCO 2015) and health policies (High et al. 2014). The aim of this study is to provide early childhood professionals with contextual information about why some parents do not read to their children that will enhance their early intervention and prevention practices.

This study had three aims: first, to document parent–child book reading behaviours at ages 2, 4 and 6 years; second, to investigate psychosocial risk factors for the absence of parent–child book reading at home; and third, to investigate the cumulative impact of psychosocial risk factors on the absence of parent–child book reading at home.

Methods

Participants

The study sample was comprised of children who participated in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC) at ages 2, 4 and 6.

The LSAC is a national longitudinal study that commenced in 2004 (Edwards 2012; Misson and Sipthorp 2007; Soloff et al. 2005, 2006). The study uses a cross-sequential design of biennial face-to-face visits with the family and study child.

In this study, we used data from the Baby Cohort collected across three waves. The ages of the children at each wave are described in Table 1. For simplicity, we refer to the children at the three waves as aged 2, 4 and 6 years. The Baby Cohort comprised 4606 children at 2 years, 4386 children at 4 years and 4242 children at 6 years.

Table 1 Sample size at each wave, children’s ages

The LSAC sampling frame was extracted from the Medicare Australia enrolment database. The initial study sample was designed to be representative of Australian children within the selected age cohort, proportional to the regional distribution of children in the Australian population (Soloff et al. 2005, 2006).

The study entailed a two-stage clustered design, first selecting postcodes then children within postcodes. Stratification was used to ensure proportional geographic representation for states/territories and capital city statistical division/rest of state areas.

Analyses show that the initial sample was broadly representative of the general Australian population when compared with 2001 Census data, but slightly under-representative of families who were single-parent, non-English speaking and living in rental properties. Attrition somewhat increased these biases (Misson and Sipthorp 2007; Taylor et al. 2013).

The analytic sample for the analyses in this paper varies because of missing data at wave or item level.

Ethics

The study has ethics approval from the Australian Institute of Family Studies Ethics Committee. The Ethics Committee is registered with the Australian Health Ethics Committee, a subcommittee of the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC). Caregivers gave written informed consent to the survey. As the study children were all minors at the time these data were collected, written informed consent was obtained from the caregiver on behalf of each of the study children. The signed consent forms are retained by the field agency, the Australian Bureau of Statistics.

Measures

Outcomes

The outcome of interest in this paper is whether or not the study child was read to by their main carer or another adult in the family in the week prior to the survey. This information was collected from parent 1 (the LSAC survey respondent, typically the main carer) when the child was aged 2, 4 and 6. Parents were asked to indicate the frequency with which children were read to in the last week on a 4-point scale ranging from “none” to “every day (6–7 days)”. Reading to children is a normative behaviour, and the absence of this behaviour is a concern. In this paper, we have a particular focus on children not read to whatsoever (Table 2).

Table 2 Read to study child in last week

From ages 2 to 6, the majority of children were read to 6 or 7 days a week. A small proportion of children were not read to at all.

Two other measures of reading behaviour were also assessed in this study; the amount of time the study child is usually read to in a sitting, and the number of children’s books in the home. The duration of reading in a sitting is measured on an 8-point scale ranging from “child doesn’t like to be read to at all” to “more than 60 min” (Table 3).

Table 3 Minutes study child usually read to in a sitting

At ages 2 and 4, the most commonly reported category of reading to the study child was 6–10 min in a sitting. At age 6, the most common category was 11–15 min per sitting.

It was relatively unlikely at any age that parents would report their child doesn’t like being read to, or was read to for more than 41 min in a sitting.

The number of children’s books in the home was assessed on a 5-point scale, ranging from “none” to “more than 30”. The majority of parents reported more than 30 children’s books in the house (Table 4).

Table 4 Children’s books in house

Barriers to Reading

This study considered eight barriers to children being read to: two child factors (Indigenous study child and low child task attention), three maternal factors (elevated maternal psychological distress, low maternal education and low maternal warmth) and three family factors (mother non-English-speaking background, single mother family and low family income).

The selection of measures used in this paper was guided by the LSAC ethos, empirical and/or theoretical associations with book reading, and our previous work in the area.

For the purposes of this analysis, we dichotomised each variable into an “at-risk” group (i.e. highest risk group) and a reference group (i.e. lowest risk group).

Study child minority race has previously been identified as an independent risk factors for low receptive language ability. A small proportion of children were of Indigenous (Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander) descent and were coded to distinguish them from those who were not.

Task attentiveness refers to children’s ability to regulate their attention in order to persist with tasks without distraction. Sawyer et al. (2015) described a method for deriving task attentiveness in their paper. Task attentiveness was measured with a total of six items at age 2 (scores ranging 6–33) and five items at age 4 and age 6 (scores ranging 5–27). We defined children in the bottom 15 % at each age as having “low task attentiveness”.

In this study, we used the Kessler-6 (K6) scale to measure maternal non-specific psychological distress. Women with scores of eight or more were classified as having symptomatic psychological distress (i.e. classified as high risk). This threshold is consistent with other studies (Lawrence et al. 2011; Strazdins et al. 2007, 2010) using the K6.

In Australia, at the time of this study, 10 years of education was compulsorily mandated. Mothers with year 11 or less were considered at risk.

The parenting warmth of the mother was measured in a self-complete form developed for the LSAC (Zubrick et al. 2008, 2014). Maternal warmth was measured at each wave by 6 questions (e.g. “In the last 6 months, how often did you feel close to the child both when he/she was happy and when he/she was upset?”). Responses to each item were on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “almost never” to “always/almost always”. Items for each measure were summed to create a composite score with higher levels representing more positive parenting characteristics. Children of mothers in the lowest 15 % of parenting warmth were considered at risk.

As the focus of this study was explicitly on English language development and because language development is known to vary where more than one language is spoken in the home, we used the mother’s non-English-speaking background (NESB) as a general indicator for language other than English spoken in the household at 4 years.

Single mother families were considered at risk because these parents face a number of resource challenges.

Families were asked to report their total weekly family income from all sources. Families in the lowest 15 % at each age were considered at risk.

Each risk factor was dichotomised, which allowed the estimation of the total number of risks each child was exposed to at ages 2, 4 and 6. This gave each child a potential number of risks from 0 to 8 at each age.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted in SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc. 2002–2012). Logistic regression was used to estimate the unadjusted and adjusted odds of children not being read to at ages 2, 4 and 6. Logistic regression was used to estimate the unadjusted odds ratios (ORs) and adjusted odds ratios (aORs) of children not being read to at ages 2, 4 and 6. Odds ratios are a relative measure of effect which allows a comparison to be made between the groups classified as “at risk” on any measured variable relative to the comparison group of “not at risk”. The odds ratios in these analyses indicate the relative likelihood that that parent–child book reading does not occur in the “at-risk” group. The adjusted odds ratios (aOR) control for any additional variables included in any logistic regression model.

Results

The distribution of risks for not being read to at ages 2, 4 and 6 is described in Table 5. Risks ranged in prevalence from Indigenous status (4.5 %) to maternal education year 11 school or less (29.6 % at child age 2).

Table 5 Barriers to reading, ages 2, 4 and 6

Table 6 describes the adjusted associations between the individual risk factors and the study child not being read to. At age 2, maternal NESB status (aOR: 4.0), low maternal education (aOR: 2.8), study child Indigeneity (aOR: 2.8), low task attention (aOR: 2.4) and low family income status (aOR: 1.7) were all associated with increased likelihood of the study child not being read to.

Table 6 Adjusted odds ratios, at ages 2, 4 and 6

At age 4, study child Indigeneity (aOR: 2.7), maternal education (aOR: 2.6), maternal NESB status (aOR: 2.3), low maternal warmth (aOR: 1.8), single mother family status (aOR: 1.8), low task attention (aOR: 1.7) and low family income status (aOR: 1.6) were all associated with increased likelihood of the study child not being read to.

At age 6, maternal education (aOR: 2.0), low maternal warmth (aOR: 1.8) and maternal NESB status (aOR: 1.4) were all associated with increased likelihood of the study child not being read to at age 6.

The pattern of statistical significance for individual risk factors varied between ages, but confidence intervals tended to overlap and little can be said about the changing pattern of significance between ages.

The distribution of risk exposures is different between children read to and not read to at ages 2, 4 and 6 (Figs. 1, 2, 3, respectively). Only 9.6 % of children not read to at age 2 were exposed to 0 risks, compared with 38.9 % of children read to at least once. 62.1 % of children not read to at age 2 experienced 2 or more risks, compared with only 25.9 % of children read to at least once in the last week.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Number of risks by reading status at age 2

Fig. 2
figure 2

Number of risks by reading status at age 4

Fig. 3
figure 3

Number of risks by reading status at age 6

18.7 % of children not read at age 4 experienced 0 risks, compared with over 42.3 % of children read to at least once. 54.6 % of children not read to at age 4 experienced 2 or more risks, compared with only 23.7 % of children read to at least once in the last week.

20 % of children not read to at age 6 experienced 0 risks, compared with 30.1 % of children read to at least once. 52.5 % of children not read to at age 6 experienced 2 or more risks, compared with 39.4 % of children read to at least once in the last week.

Table 7 describes the average number of risks for children at ages 2, 4 and 6 by their reading status. Children not read to at age 2 were exposed to an average of 2.1 risks, compared with an average of 1.0 risks per child read to in the past week at age 2. Children not read to at age 4 were exposed to an average of 1.9 risks, compared with an average of 0.9 risks per child read to in the past week at age 4. Children not read to at age 6 were exposed to an average of 1.5 risks, compared with an average of 1.0 risks per child read to in the past week at age 6. The difference between children who were read to in the last week and children not read to whatsoever in the last week was statistically significant at each age.

Table 7 Distribution of risks by reading status at ages 2, 4, and 6

There was an increased likelihood of not being read at each age as children were exposed an increasing number of risks. At age 2, being exposed to a single risk increased the likelihood of not being read to by an odds ratio of 3.3. Those children exposed to four or more risks had an odds ratio 21.4 relative to children exposed to zero risks at age 2.

At age 4, being exposed to a single risk increased the likelihood of not being read to by an odds ratio of 1.8. Those children exposed to four or more risks had an odds ratio 12.4 relative to children exposed to zero risks at age 4.

There was an increased likelihood of not being read at age 6 to as children were exposed an increasing number of risks, although there was no statistically significant difference between children exposed to zero and one risk at age 6. Being exposed to a two risk increased the likelihood of not being read to by an odds ratio of 1.5. Those children exposed to four or more risks had an odds ratio 3.9 relative to children exposed to zero risks at age 6 (Table 8).

Table 8 Odds ratios for not being read to by cumulative risks at ages 2, 4 and 6

Discussion

This study had three aims: first, to document parent–child book reading behaviours at ages 2, 4 and 6 years; second, to investigate psychosocial risk factors for the absence of parent–child book reading at home; and third, to investigate the cumulative impact of psychosocial risk factors on the absence of parent–child book reading at home. From ages 2 to 6, some 6–8 % of children were not read to at all. While the impact of individual risk factors varied across ages, cumulative risk mattered.

The study identified a group of children where exposure to multiple risks across all domains was a likely experience. This is consistent with other studies. Other authors have also found that economic disadvantage may co-occur with other risks, including limited social support, parental mental and physical health, educational level and intellectual ability, attitudes and cognitive readiness to parent coping style (Guralnick 2013). Sabates and Dex (2012) describe the distribution of ten risks within the UK Millennium Child Cohort Study. These risks included depression, physical disability, lack of basic skills, parental smoking during pregnancy, domestic violence and financial stress. They found that 28 % of families with young children were facing two or more risks, with a smaller proportion of families exposed to numerous risks. Those children living in families with low income and multiple risks fared worse across cognitive development and social-emotional development at ages 3 and 5 than their peers at lower risk.

In an Australian context, Biddle and Seth-Purdie (2013) also considered multiple risk factors within the LSAC. Although they did not consider distinct groups within the sample, they found that children exposed to increasing numbers of risks were more likely to have low teacher-rated academic achievement, maths and literacy, and worse psychological wellbeing.

Poverty and overlapping risks are associated with worse cognitive and educational outcomes. Ayoub et al. (2009) found that children in poverty who live in comprehensively riskier systems have worse cognitive outcomes in the preschool-age groups. The current approach also highlights the complexities of overlapping patterns of child and family risk factors that are barriers to book reading. Results of meta-analyses of parent–child book reading interventions have revealed heterogeneity in effect sizes, favouring children from higher socio-economic households (Manz et al. 2010; Mol et al. 2008). The findings from the current study suggest that addressing the absence of shared book reading requires a holistic approach that addresses the multiple risk contexts that explain why some parents do not read to their children.

The policy implications of our findings require further exploration. On the one hand, it is helpful to know that a child who is not read to is more likely to be exposed to multiple risks. On the other hand, it is not immediately clear how one would identify which specific mix of risks a child is likely to be exposed to, on the basis of not being read to. Our previous work has shown that a well-fitted multivariate model does not necessarily predict child language outcomes across time with great precision, and we have grappled with the implications of this finding (Christensen et al. 2014; Zubrick et al. 2015). This issue remains unresolved in the current paper.

Our work highlights the need for services to take a holistic approach, with our key finding a remarkable overlap between child, maternal and family circumstances including child abilities, maternal psychological wellbeing and parenting style, cultural factors and socio-economic circumstances including income, education, employment and neighbourhood conditions.

In the Australian context, several programmes aim at helping families exposed to multiple disadvantage (Hilferty and Redmond 2009; McDonald et al. 2012; Taylor et al. 2015). The Home Interaction Program for Parents and Youngsters (HIPPY) is specifically targeted at children from disadvantaged communities and aims to improve their school readiness over a 2-year period concluding with the end of their first year at school (Lidedell et al. 2009, 2011). This programme uses a mix of home- and centre-based early childhood enrichments to help parents in building their children’s school readiness. The Tasmanian Child and Family Centre programme takes a place-based approach to multiple disadvantage, offering a range of services to families experiencing disadvantage (Taylor et al. 2015). The Maternal Early Childhood Sustained Home-visiting (MECSH) programme is a structured programme of sustained nurse home visiting for families at risk of poorer maternal and child health and development outcomes. These targeted, place-based programmes are voluntary. Families are enrolled through word of mouth or recommendation from agencies. Access is usually determined by location. For example, HIPPY sites were selected based on measures of neighbourhood disadvantage and demographics, and the capacity of a local service provider and links with other programmes.

Internationally, there are a range of early childhood interventions aimed at addressing multiple disadvantage. Comparing these programmes is difficult because of the heterogeneity of approaches, but Guralnick (2013) reviews the characteristics of successful early childhood interventions. While the most consistent findings indicated the value of centre-based programmes for preschool-age children in the cognitive domain, these effects tend to be modest and to fade over time. He struggled to identify consistent patterns of success in home visiting programmes. Guralnick’s (2013) own Developmental Systems Approach explicitly acknowledges the mix of children’s social and cognitive competence, family patterns of interaction, family resources, and the interactions between them.

One take-away for service providers is to look broadly when considering patterns of risks—child, maternal and family risk factors may co-occur in complex patterns.

The results support early home learning programmes that address psychosocial risk factors that are barriers to active engagement in home learning activities such as parent–child book reading. The findings suggest that universal parent–child book reading interventions that focus solely on home learning activities may not meet the needs of families in which the absence of parent–child book reading signals a multiplicity of risk factors for child development.

Conclusions

For early childhood education and care service providers, asking parents how often they read to their children is an important question. In this study, the absence of book reading was a red flag that signalled the potential for a multiplicity of psychosocial risk factors in the child’s home learning environment. Family-centred practice aims to support and strengthen child health, development and learning, starting with mutually agreed intervention priorities (Knoche et al. 2012). For parents who do not read to their children, their highest priority may not be reading to their children. For example, there may be other stressors or family priorities associated with the risk factors identified that prevent parents from devoting time to read to their child.

The design and delivery of parent–child book reading interventions needs to be grounded in knowledge about the psychosocial hazards associated with the absence of book reading and sensitivity to the multiple disadvantages and challenging circumstances that beset many families and children.