Skip to main content
Log in

Deriving Features of Religions in the Wild

How Communication and Threat-Detection May Predict Spirits, Gods, Witches, and Shamans

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Religions “in the wild” are the varied set of religious activities that occurred before the emergence of organized religions with doctrines, or that persist at the margins of those organized traditions. These religious activities mostly focus on misfortune; on how to remedy specific cases of illness, accidents, failures; and on how to prevent them. I present a general model to account for the cross-cultural recurrence of these particular themes. The model is based on (independently established) features of human psychology—namely, (a) epistemic vigilance, the set of systems whereby we evaluate the quality of information and of sources of information, and (b) threat-detection psychology, the set of evolved systems geared at detecting potential danger in the environment. Given these two sets of systems, the dynamics of communication will favor particular types of messages about misfortune. This makes it possible to predict recurrent features of religious systems, such as the focus on nonphysical agents, the focus on particular cases rather than general aspects of misfortune, and the emergence of specialists. The model could illuminate not just why such representations are culturally successful, but also why people are motivated to formulate them in the first place.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For instance, in a classic study, Ron Brunton (1980) pointed out that students of Melanesian religious representations produced coherent general principles from inconsistent specific comments made by informants, which suggests that Melanesian religions are “weakly integrated . . . subject to a high degree of individual variation and a high rate of innovation and obsolescence” (1980:112). In the same way, Evans-Pritchard states that talking about a “Zande theology” would be a gross distortion of Zande religious activities, which neither strive for nor achieve consistency beyond very general statements about spirits and witchcraft (Evans-Pritchard, 1936, 1963). In the same spirit, Rodney Needham argued that statements of causality (the spirits caused x) are not accompanied by representations of causal processes (Needham, 1976).

  2. For instance, this approach has provided us with models and hypotheses on such aspects of religions as concepts of gods and spirits and other superhuman agents (Atran, 2002; Boyer, 1994a; Pyysiainen, 2001); notions of death and immortality (McCorkle, 2010; White, 2016); recurrent features of rituals (Lawson and McCauley, 1990; Liénard and Boyer, 2006); connections between differences in religious doctrines and differences in cooperation (Baumard et al., 2015; Purzycki et al., 2016)—for general surveys, see Barrett, 2000; Boyer and Bergstrom, 2008; Xygalatas, 2014.

  3. This of course assumes an idealized game dynamic, a limiting case in which there is only time for one foraging expedition given the alternatives described.

  4. Note that in this model, people are more likely to follow a recommendation if it emanates from a source that has prior Good Source status, independently of that specific recommendation. This is the general phenomenon studied in formal models of advice-taking (Pornpitakpan, 2004; Sobel, 2013), distinct from those highly specific situations, in which an agent produces both a statement and a behavior that would be risky if the statement was false, therefore boosting their credibility (Henrich, 2009).

  5. This is not an exceptional situation in reaction to danger and misfortune. In a somewhat similar manner, people throughout history agreed to undergo medical treatments that brought about no clear benefits. Apart from some actually effective cures, based for example on empirical knowledge of plants, medicine until the beginning of the twentieth century had very limited therapeutic efficacy and in fact often compromised the patients’ recovery (e.g., in the case of bloodletting). Nevertheless, few people considered it a viable option to ignore medical prescriptions and let nature take its course.

  6. Note, again, the persistence of such dynamics in attitudes to medicine, even in modern contexts where practitioners make use of general findings from biology. In their interaction with patients, physicians do not derive their authority from their knowledge of the general facts of physiology and anatomy—which many biologists possess too, and sometimes in greater detail and accuracy. The authority of doctors is linked to clinical competence—that is, the presumed capacity to identify the unique features of each pathological case.

  7. Obviously, there may be reasons, other than Good Source dynamics that lead to this focus. For one thing, people may focus on the particular case because they need a consensus view on who might be responsible for the particular situation at hand (Boyer, 2020).

References

  • Atran, S. A. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R. (1994). Sources of variation in ethnographic interview data: Food avoidances in the Ituri Forest, Zaire. Ethnology, 33(1), 65–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 29–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumard, N., Hyafil, A., Morris, I., & Boyer, P. (2015). Increased affluence explains the emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions. Current Biology, 25, 10–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.063

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beard, M. (1996). The Roman and the foreign: The cult of the “Great Mother” in Imperial Rome. In N. Thomas & C. Humphrey (Eds.), Shamanism, history and the state (pp. 164–188). University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bebbington, K., MacLeod, C., Ellison, T. M., & Fay, N. (2017). The sky is falling: Evidence of a negativity bias in the social transmission of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 92–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellah, R. N. (2011). Religion in human evolution: From the Paleolithic to the axial age. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C., & Fukui, Y. (1999). The great illusion: Ignorance, informational cascades, and the persistence of unpopular norms. In M. C. Galavotti & A. Pagnani (Eds.), Experience, reality, and scientific explanation (pp. 89–121). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bikhchandani, S., Hirshleifer, D., & Welch, I. (1998). Learning from the behavior of others: Conformity, fads, and informational cascades. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12(3), 151–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaine, T., & Boyer, P. (2018). Origins of sinister rumors: A preference for threat-related material in the supply and demand of information. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 67–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.10.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonaccio, S., & Dalal, R. S. (2010). Evaluating advisors: A policy-capturing study under conditions of complete and missing information. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23(3), 227–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rare. In W. G. Runciman, J. M. Smith, & et al. (Eds.), Evolution of social behaviour patterns in primates and man. (pp. 77–93). Oxford University Press.

  • Boyer, P. (1993). Pseudo-natural kinds. In P. Boyer (Ed.), Cognitive aspects of religious symbolism (pp. 121–141). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (1994a). Cognitive constraints on cultural representations: Natural ontologies and religious ideas. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain-specificity in culture and cognition (pp. 391–411). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (1994b). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion. University of California Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: Evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2018). Minds make societies: How cognition explains the world humans create. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2019). Informal religious activity outside hegemonic religions: Wild traditions and their relevance to evolutionary models. Religion, Brain & Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2019.1678518

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (2020). Why divination? Evolved psychology and strategic interaction in the production of truth. Current Anthropology, 61(1), 100–123. https://doi.org/10.1086/706879

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2008). Evolutionary perspectives on religion. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 111–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2011). Threat-detection in child development: An evolutionary perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1034–1041.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 595–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Parren, N. (2015). Threat-related information suggests competence: A possible factor in the spread of rumors. PLoS One, 10(6), e0128421. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. (1981). The cult of the saints: Its rise and function in Latin Christianity. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunton, R. (1980). Misconstrued order in Melanesian religion. Man, 15(1), new series, 112–128. https://doi.org/10.2307/2802005.

  • Claidière, N., Amedon, G.K.-K., André, J.-B., Kirby, S., Smith, K., Sperber, D., et al. (2018). Convergent transformation and selection in cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.12.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Sperber, D. (2014). How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0368

  • Clément, F. (2010). To trust or not to trust? Children’s social epistemology. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1, 531–549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-010-0022-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, J. C. (1985). Vital souls: Bororo cosmology, natural symbolism and shamanism. University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dengah, H. F. (2013). The contract with God: Patterns of cultural consensus across two Brazilian religious communities. Journal of Anthropological Research, 69(3), 347–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dressler, W. W. (2020). Cultural consensus and cultural consonance: Advancing a cognitive theory of culture. Field Methods, 32(4), 383–398. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X20935599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1935). Witchcraft. Africa, 8(4), 417–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1936). Zande theology. Sudan Notes and Records, 19(1), 5–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1937). Witchcraft, oracles and magic among the Azande. Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1963). Essays in social anthropology. Free Press of Glencoe.

  • Favret-Saada, J. (1980). Deadly words: Witchcraft in the bocage. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fessler, D. M. T., Pisor, A. C., & Navarrete, C. D. (2014). Negatively-biased credulity and the cultural evolution of beliefs. PLoS One, 9, e95167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortes, M. (1966). Religious premisses and logical technique in divinatory ritual. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 251, 409–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortes, M. (1987). Religion, morality and the person: Essays on Tallensi religion. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fuller, C. J. (1992). The camphor flame: Popular Hinduism and society in India. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gellner, E. (1981). Muslim society. Cambridge University Press.

  • Goody, J. (1972). The myth of the Bagre. Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., & Lane, J. D. (2014). Infants understand how testimony works. Topoi, 33, 443–458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-013-9180-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 81–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 244–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.03.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution & Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilbig, B. E. (2009). Sad, thus true: Negativity bias in judgments of truth. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 983–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilbig, B. E. (2012). How framing statistical statements affects subjective veracity: Validation and application of a multinomial model for judgments of truth. Cognition, 125(1), 37–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (1999). How biological is essentialism? In S. Atran & M. Douglas (Eds.), Folk-Biology (pp. 403–446). The M.I.T. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horton, W. R. G. (1967). African traditional thought and Western science. Africa, 37(2), 50–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. D., Blumstein, D. T., Fowler, J. H., & Haselton, M. G. (2013). The evolution of error: Error management, cognitive constraints, and adaptive decision-making biases. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 28(8), 474–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A., & Bateson, M. (1997). Risk-sensitivity: Crossroads for theories of decision-making. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(8), 304–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1991). Anomalies: The endowment effect, loss aversion, and status quo bias. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 5, 193–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keesing, R. M. (1982). Kwaio religion: The living and the dead in a Solomon Island society. Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuran, T. (1998). Ethnic norms and their transformation through reputational cascades. Journal of Legal Studies, 27, 623–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, E. T., & McCauley, R. N. (1990). Rethinking religion: Connecting cognition and culture. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Bras, G. (1956). Études de sociologie religieuse. Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, I. H. (1993). On the convergence of informational cascades. Journal of Economic Theory, 61(2), 395–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, I. M. (1966). Spirit possession and deprivation cults. Man, 1, 307–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liénard, P., & Boyer, P. (2006). Whence collective rituals? A cultural selection model of ritualized behavior. American Anthropologist, 108, 814–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lienhardt, G. (1987). Divinity and experience: The religion of the Dinka. Oxford University Press.

  • Luhrmann, T. M. (1989). Persuasions of the witch’s craft. Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mascaro, O., & Morin, O. (2014). Gullible’s travel: How honest and trustful children become vigilant communicators. In E. J. Robinson, S. Einav, E. J. Robinson, & S. Einav (Eds.), Trust and skepticism: Children’s selective learning from testimony (pp. 69–82). Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mascaro, O., & Sperber, D. (2009). The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition, 112, 367–380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCorkle, W. W. (2010). Ritualizing the disposal of the deceased : From corpse to concept (Toronto Studies in Religion, 30). Peter Lang.

  • McCutcheon, R. T. (1997). Manufacturing religion: The discourse on sui generis religion and the politics of nostalgia. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. (2012). The social functions of explicit coherence evaluation. Mind & Society, 11, 81–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-011-0095-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2017). The enigma of reason. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, S., & Fiddick, L. (2012). Beyond gains and losses: The effect of need on risky choice in framed decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, S., Gregson, M., & Lalumiere, M. L. (2012). Framing effects and risk-sensitive decision making. British Journal of Psychology, 103(1), 83–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Needham, R. (1976). Skulls and Causality. Man, 11, 71–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, S. M. (2012). New Age and neopagan religions in America. Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Platvoet, J. G. (1993). African traditional religions in the religious history of mankind. Journal for the Study of Religion, 6(2), 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 243–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., et al. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature, 530, 327–330. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16980

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pyysiainen, I. (2001). How religion works: Towards a new cognitive science of religion. Brill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1991). Neo-conditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisition. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 155–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(91)90093-a

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, R. A. (1979). Ecology, meaning and religion. North Atlantic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redfield, R. (1989). The little community and peasant society and culture. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricci, D. (2014). Japanese shamanism: Trance and possession. Volume Press. (digital version available online at www.amazon.com).

  • Rode, C., Cosmides, L., Hell, W., & Tooby, J. (1999). When and why do people avoid unknown probabilities in decisions under uncertainty? Testing some predictions from optimal foraging theory. Cognition, 72, 269–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romney, A. K., Weller, S. C., & Batchelder, W. H. (1986). Culture as consensus: A theory of culture and informant accuracy. American Anthropologist, 88(2), 313–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M., & Taylor, M. (1990). Category labels and social reality: Do we view social categories as natural kinds? In G. Semin & K. Fiedler (Eds.), Language and social cognition (pp. 11–36). Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296–320. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shariff, A. F., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2010). The birth of high gods: How the cultural evolution of supernatural policing influenced the emergence of complex, cooperative human societies, paving the way for civilization. In M. Schaller, A. Norenzayan, S. J. Heine, T. Yamagishi, & T. Kameda (Eds.), Evolution, culture, and the human mind (pp. 119–136). Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, M. (2018). The cultural evolution of shamanism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 41, e66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17001893

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skorupski, J. (1975). Symbol and theory. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, J. (2013). Giving and receiving advice. Advances in Economics and Econometrics, 1, 305–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1975). Rethinking symbolism. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1985). Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man, 20, 73–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1997). Intuitive and reflective beliefs. Mind and Language, 12, 67–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (2000). Metarepresentation in an evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 117–137). Vancouver Studies in Cognitive Science, 10. Oxford University Press

  • Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 40–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., et al. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25, 359–393. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, H. F. (1974). Envy and the evil eye among Slovak-Americans: An essay in the psychological ontogeny of belief and ritual. Ethos, 2, 15–46. https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1974.2.1.02a00020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stépanoff, C. (2015). Transsingularities: The cognitive foundations of shamanism in Northern Asia. Social Anthropology, 23(2), 169–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, D. W., & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging theory. Princeton University Press.

  • Stewart, C. (1991). Demons and the devil: Moral imagination in modern Greek culture. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tambiah, S. J. (1970). Buddhism and the spirit cults in north-east Thailand. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & et al. (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. (pp. 19-136). Oxford University Press

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (Eds.). (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermaseren, M. J. (1977). Cybele and Attis: The myth and the cult. Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vullioud, C., Clément, F., Scott-Phillips, T., & Mercier, H. (2017). Confidence as an expression of commitment: Why misplaced expressions of confidence backfire. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 9–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.06.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vyse, S. A. (1997). Believing in magic: The psychology of superstition. University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (2000). Conditioned emotional reactions. American Psychologist, 55, 313–317. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.55.3.313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, C. (2016). The cognitive foundations of reincarnation. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 28, 264–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, H. (2000). Arguments and icons. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of religiosity. Altamira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkelman, M. J. (1986). Magico-religious practitioner types and socioeconomic conditions. Behavior Science Research, 20, 17–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkelman, M. J. (1990). Shamans and other “magico-religious” healers: A cross-cultural study of their origins, nature, and social transformations. Ethos, 18, 308–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkelman, M. J., & White, D. (1987). A cross-cultural study of magico-religious practitioners and trance states: Database. In D. Levinson (Ed.), HRAF Research Series in Quantitative Cross-Cultural Data. Human Relations Area Files.

  • Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person- and self-perception. European Review of Social Psychology, 16(1), 155–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xygalatas, D. (2014). Cognitive science of religion. In D. A. Leeming (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology and religion (pp. 343–347). Springer.

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author is grateful to Niklas Andersson, Nicolas Baumard, Pierre Liénard, Hugo Mercier, Olivier Morin, Manvir Singh, Human Nature’s editor and anonymous reviewers for their comments on a previous version.

Funding

Research for this article was funded by a planning grant from the Templeton Religion Trust (#TRT0295).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pascal Boyer.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Boyer, P. Deriving Features of Religions in the Wild. Hum Nat 32, 557–581 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09410-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09410-y

Keywords

Navigation