Skip to main content
Log in

The Conundrum of Modern Art

Prestige-Driven Coevolutionary Aesthetics Trumps Evolutionary Aesthetics among Art Experts

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two major mechanisms of aesthetic evolution have been suggested. One focuses on naturally selected preferences (Evolutionary Aesthetics), while the other describes a process of evaluative coevolution whereby preferences coevolve with signals. Signaling theory suggests that expertise moderates these mechanisms. In this article we set out to verify this hypothesis in the domain of art and use it to elucidate Western modern art’s deviation from naturally selected preferences. We argue that this deviation is consistent with a Coevolutionary Aesthetics mechanism driven by prestige-biased social learning among art experts. In order to test this hypothesis, we conducted two studies in which we assessed the effects on lay and expert appreciation of both the biological relevance of the given artwork’s depicted content, viz., facial beauty, and the prestige specific to the artwork’s associated context (MoMA). We found that laypeople appreciate artworks based on their depictions of facial beauty, mediated by aesthetic pleasure, which is consistent with previous studies. In contrast, experts appreciate the artworks based on the prestige of the associated context, mediated by admiration for the artist. Moreover, experts appreciate artworks depicting neutral faces to a greater degree than artworks depicting attractive faces. These findings thus corroborate our contention that expertise moderates the Evolutionary and Coevolutionary Aesthetics mechanisms in the art domain. Furthermore, our findings provide initial support for our proposal that prestige-driven coevolution with expert evaluations plays a decisive role in modern art’s deviation from naturally selected preferences. After discussing the limitations of our research as well as the relation that our results bear on cultural evolution theory, we provide a number of suggestions for further research into the potential functions of expert appreciation that deviates from naturally selected preferences, on the one hand, and expertise as a moderator of these mechanisms in other cultural domains, on the other.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that this latter process of evaluative coevolution can proceed by genetic and/or cultural mechanisms. Where the art expert is concerned, this process depends on cultural mechanisms.

  2. Even though bootstrapping is becoming the most popular method for testing mediation (Hayes 2009), we have chosen to use the Sobel test when testing mediation of within-subjects effects given that, to the best of our knowledge, no published bootstrapping method of such effects exists (Andrew F. Hayes, personal communication; Zhao et al. 2010). Moreover, our samples are large enough that they are not vulnerable to the typical problems associated with the Sobel test.

  3. In both studies, subjective expertise was higher among female participants (Study 1a: M = 20.39, SD = 6.42; Study 1b: M = 19.87, SD = 7.78) than it was among male participants (Study 1a: M = 14.76, SD = 6.09; Study 1b: M = 15.55, SD = 6.48). Study 1a: F 1,150 = 30.56, p < 0.01; Study 1b: F 1,119 = 10.13, p < 0.01.

  4. In addition, as this question was asked of all participants, it allowed us to verify whether experts were better than laypeople at identifying that our stimuli did not belong to the MoMA. A z test to compare two proportions revealed that experts (p = 3.8%) and laypeople (p = 8%) performed equally poorly in distinguishing between real MoMA artwork used as fillers and the face research pictures used as stimuli (z = 1.3, p = 0.11).

  5. GLMM on the total sample with the continuous expertise variable yielded results that were very similar to the expertise grouping variable. It revealed the predicted significant interactions between expertise and prestige, F 1,189 = 3.90, p = 0.05, and between expertise and content, F 1,189 = 34.70, p < 0.01. In addition, the analysis indicated a significant main effect of expertise, F 1,189 = 18.34, p < 0.01 and of content, F 1,189 = 4.062, p = 0.05.

  6. Adding gender to the model showed that the content effect was partially moderated by gender (F 1,185 = 14.83, p < 0.01): as simple contrast tests indicated, men appreciated pictures of attractive faces (M = 4.16, SD = 1.15) more than those of neutral faces (M = 3.78, SD = 1.35; F 1,185 = 22.31, p < 0.01) because all other contrasts were not significant. This simple effect of men is likely due to the fact that we used pictures of women’s faces. Gender did not moderate the interactions that are of interest to us: expertise and content (F 1,185 = 0.47, p = 0.49) and expertise and prestige (F 1,185 = 0.09, p = 0.76).

  7. Although mediated moderation models may seem at first glance a more suitable approach to analyzing data such as ours, to the best of our knowledge, mediated moderation models that can handle a mixed design (i.e., both within- and between-subject factors) have not yet been developed. Consequently, we resorted to the simple, more traditional mediation analyses.

References

  • Arak, A., & Enquist, M. (1995). Conflict, receiver bias and the evolution of signal form. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 349, 337–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkisson, C., O’Brien, M. J., & Mesoudi, A. (2012). Adult learners in a novel environment use prestige biased social learning. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 519–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C. (2005). Adaptations to predators and prey. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 200–223). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basil, M. D. (1996). Identification as a mediator of celebrity effects. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 40(4), 478–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. V. (2013). Evolutionary thinking in microeconomic models: prestige bias and market bubbles. PloS One, 8(3), e59805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1979). Le sens commun: la distinction critique sociale du jugement. Paris: Les Editions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullot, N. J., & Reber, R. (2013). The artful mind meets art history: toward a psycho-historical framework for the science of art appreciation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 123–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7, 89–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornelissen, G., Dewitte, S., Warlop, L., & Yserbyt, V. (2007). Whatever people say I am, that’s what I am: social labeling as a social marketing tool. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 24, 278–288.

  • Cupchik, G. C., & Laszlo, J. (1992). Emerging visions of the aesthetic process. Psychology, semiology and philosophy. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danto, A. (1964). The artworld. Journal of Philosophy, 61, 571–584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danto, A. (2003). The abuse of beauty: Aesthetics and the concept of art. Chicago: Open Court.

  • Davies, S. (2012). The artful species. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • de Jager, M. L., & Ellis, A. G. (2014). Costs of deception and learned resistance in deceptive interactions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20132861.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Nooy, W. (2002). The dynamics of artistic prestige. Poetics, 30, 147–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, E. (1995). Homo aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

  • Dissanayake, E. (1998). Komar and Melamid discover Pleistocene taste. Philosophy and Literature, 22(2), 486–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, D. (1998). America’s most wanted, and why no one wants it. Philosophy and Literature, 22(2), 530–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egger, B., Klaefiger, Y., Theis, A., & Salzburger, W. (2011). A sensory bias has triggered the evolution of egg-spots in cichlid fishes. PloS One, 6(10), e25601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J. A., & Basolo, A. L. (1998). Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 13, 415–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Arak, A., Ghirlanda, S., & Wachtmeister, C. A. (2002). Spectacular phenomena and limits to rationality in genetic and cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 357, 1585–1594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. H., & Balling, J. D. (2010). Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference. Environment and Behavior, 42, 479–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection: A complete variorum edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Garcia, C. M., & Ramirez, E. (2005). Evidence that sensory traps can evolve into honest signals. Nature, 434(7032), 501–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geyskens, K., Dewitte, S., Pandelaere, M., & Warlop, L. (2008). Tempt me just a little bit more: the effect of food temptation actionability on goal activation and subsequent consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 600–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420.

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2002). On modeling cognition and culture. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2, 87–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366(1567), 1139–1148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D., & Watson, B. (2015). The visual brain and the early depiction of animals in Europe and Southeast Asia. World Archaeology, 47(5), 1–16.

  • Horner, V., Proctor, D., Bonnie, K. E., & Whiten, A. (2010). Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLoS One. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010625.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, R. (2002). Signalling theory: signal design and selection for efficient displays, coevolution between signaller and receiver. In M. Pagel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Kampe, K. K., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, U. (2001). Reward value of attractiveness and gaze. Nature, 413(6856), 589–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkpatrick, M. (1982). Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution, 36, 1–12.

  • Kirkpatrick, M., & Ryan, M. J. (1991). The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek. Nature, 350, 33–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Komar, V., Melamid, A., & Wypijewski, J. (1997). Painting by numbers: Komar and Melamid’s scientific guide to art. Oakland: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lande, R. (1981). Models of speciation by sexual selection on polygenic traits. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 78, 3721–3725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leder, H., Gerger, G., Dressler, S., & Schabmann, A. (2012). How art is appreciated. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 2–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A., Jones, B., & DeBruine, L. (2011). Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366(1571), 1638–1659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. H. (1995). A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 30, 41–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, J. P., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). The emotional origins of a social learning bias: does the pride expression cue copying? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4, 492–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A. (2007). Using the methods of experimental social psychology to study cultural evolution. Journal of Social, Evolutionary and Cultural Psychology, 1(2), 35–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind. London: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, O. (2013). How portraits turned their eyes upon us: visual preferences and demographic change in cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 222–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, K., Kawashima, R., Nagumo, S., Ito, K., Sugiura, M., Kato, T., et al. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of the assessment of facial attractiveness. NeuroReport, 9, 753–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 16598–16603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 555–579). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prum, R. O. (2010). The Lande–Kirkpatrick mechanism is the null model of evolution by intersexual selection: implications for meaning, honesty, and design in intersexual signals. Evolution, 64, 3085–3100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prum, R. O. (2012). Aesthetic evolution by mate choice: Darwin’s really dangerous idea. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367(1600), 2253–2265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prum, R. O. (2013). Coevolutionary aesthetics in human and biotic artworlds. Biology & Philosophy, 28(5), 811–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-Garcia, V., Molina, J. L., Broesch, J., Calvet, L., Huanca, T., Saus, J., & McDade, T. W. (2008). Do the aged and knowledgeable men enjoy more prestige? A test of predictions from the prestige-bias model of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 275–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M. J. (1998). Sexual selection, receiver biases, and the evolution of sex differences. Science, 281, 1999–2003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacht, A., Werheid, K., & Sommer, W. (2008). The appraisal of facial beauty is rapid but not mandatory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(2), 132–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290–312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 40–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiner, W. (2001). Venus in exile: The rejection of beauty in 20th-century art. New York: Free Press.

  • Thompson, D. (2009). The $12 million stuffed shark: The curious economics of contemporary art. Toronto: Anchor Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics informs traditional aesthetics. In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 9–38). Berlin: Springer.

  • Van de Cruys, S., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Putting reward in art: a tentative prediction error account of visual art. i-Perception, 2(9), 1035–1062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verpooten, J., & Nelissen, M. (2010). Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: the late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory in Biosciences, 129(2–3), 211–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verpooten, J., Nelissen, M. (2012). Sensory exploitation: underestimated in the evolution of art as once in sexual selection? In K.S. Plaisance, T.A.C. Reydon (Eds.), Philosophy of behavioral biology (pp. 189–216), Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science 282(4). Dordrecht: Springer.

  • Voland, E., & Grammer, K. (2003). Evolutionary aesthetics. Berlin: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Windhager, S., Atzwanger, K., Bookstein, F. L., & Schaefer, K. (2011). Fish in a mall aquarium—an ethological investigation of biophilia. Landscape and Urban Planning, 99, 23–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winston, J. S., O’Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 45, 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, T. (1975). The painted word. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, J., Wang, A., Yan, M., Zhu, Z., Chen, C., & Wang, Y. (2012). Distinct processing for pictures of animals and objects: evidence from eye movements. Emotion, 12(3), 540–551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, X., Lynch Jr., J., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to the members of the Behavioral Engineering Group Research and the Centre for Marketing and Consumer Science at the University of Leuven for commenting on these studies on various occasions. Thanks as well to the members of the Ethology Group at the University of Antwerp and The Centre for Logic and Analytic Philosophy at the University of Leuven for their comments. Special thanks to Morgan David, Sam Franssens, and Yannick Joye for commenting on a draft of the paper, to Els Van Peborgh for discussion, to Anouk Festjens for suggestions concerning statistical analysis and to Gabi Lipede for amendments to the final draft. This paper was supported by the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO; Grant no. G085012 N).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan Verpooten.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(DOCX 1.02 mb)

Appendix

Appendix

The multiple choice art quiz consisted of ten questions. One question was the above-mentioned check concerning whether the fact that the face stimuli did not belong to the MoMA was successfully concealed; this question did not count for the expertise score. In seven of the remaining nine questions, we asked who created the visual artwork that was displayed, ranging from Renaissance art (Bruegel’s The Tower of Babel) to contemporary art (e.g., Damien Hirst’s The Physical Impossibility of Death in the Mind of Someone Living) and variously involving a painting, an installation, or a performance. One question concerned who painted the Mona Lisa and another involved placing art genres in chronological order. With the exception of the latter, all questions were multiple choice, offering 4 or 5 options, including an “I don’t know” option.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Verpooten, J., Dewitte, S. The Conundrum of Modern Art. Hum Nat 28, 16–38 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9274-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9274-7

Keywords

Navigation