Skip to main content
Log in

Geographical Cues and Developmental Exposure

Navigational Style, Wayfinding Anxiety, and Childhood Experience in the Faroe Islands

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current study assessed potential relationships among childhood wayfinding experience, navigational style, and adult wayfinding anxiety in the Faroe Islands. The Faroe Islands are of interest because they have an unusual geography that may promote the use of an orientational style of navigation (e.g., use of cardinal directions). Faroese adults completed questionnaires assessing (1) their permitted childhood range sizes, (2) the types of navigational strategies they use, and (3) the amount of anxiety they experience when navigating in adulthood. Males had more childhood wayfinding experience, used the orientation strategy at a higher rate, and showed lower levels of wayfinding anxiety. When compared with other cultures, both Faroese women and men appear to embrace orientation strategies at an unusually high rate. Childhood experience was not conclusively linked to later wayfinding anxiety. However, the current findings raise the possibility that children who have particularly small ranges in childhood may be especially anxious when navigating in adulthood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. By asking about what children were allowed to do, the current study addressed the specific question of parental permissiveness. However, the phrasing left open the possibility that children might have roamed much further without permission. This intriguing possibility is worth exploring further; however, previous research suggests that, in childhood, permitted ranges are very similar to total ranges (Herman et al. 1987). See also Hart 1979 for related sex differences.

  2. Lawton and Kallai’s sample consisted of 423 Americans (274 female) and 263 Hungarians (140 female). The original data were not available for direct statistical comparison. Therefore, comparisons are based only on the publicly available means and standard deviations.

References

  • Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20(5–6), 327–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coluccia, E., & Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24(3), 329–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Føroya, H. (2014). Faroe Islands in figures, 2014. Retrieved from http://www.hagstova.fo/sites/default/files/Faroe%20Islands%20in%20figures%202014_0.pdf.

  • Fyhri, A., Hjorthol, R., Mackett, R. L., Fotel, T. N., & Kyttä, M. (2011). Children's active travel and independent mobility in four countries: development, social contributing trends and measures. Transport Policy, 18(5), 703–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaffin, D. (1996). In place: spatial and social order in a Faeroe islands community. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaini, F. (2013). Lessons of islands: place and identity in the Faroe Islands. Fróðskapur: Faroe University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. (1979). Children's experience of place. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herman, J. F., Heins, J. A., & Cohen, D. S. (1987). Children's spatial knowledge of their neighborhood environment. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillman, M., Adams, J., & Whitelegg, J. (1990). One false move: A study of children’s independent mobility. London: Policy Studies Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kyttä, M. (1997). Children’s independent mobility in urban, small town, and rural environments. In R. Cammstra (Ed.), Growing up in a changing urban landscape (pp. 41–52). Assen: Royal Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30(11–12), 765–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A. (1996). Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16(2), 137–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A., & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 47(9–10), 389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeMoyne, T., & Buchanan, T. (2011). Does “hovering” matter? Helicopter parenting and its effect on well-being. Sociological Spectrum, 31(4), 399–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massa, L. J., Mayer, R. E., & Bohon, L. M. (2005). Individual differences in gender role beliefs influence spatial ability test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(2), 99–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, M. H. (1987). Gender, home range and environmental cognition. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 12, 43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitra, R., Faulkner, G. E., Buliung, R. N., & Stone, M. R. (2014). Do parental perceptions of the neighbourhood environment influence children’s independent mobility? Evidence from Toronto, Canada. Urban Studies, 51(16), 3401–3419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moè, A. (2009). Are males always better than females in mental rotation? Exploring a gender belief explanation. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 21–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability in an East African society. The Journal of Social Psychology, 83(1), 15–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nerlove, S. B., Munroe, R. H., & Munroe, R. L. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability: a replication. The Journal of Social Psychology, 84(1), 3–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, M., Jones, D., Sloan, D., & Rustin, M. (2000). Children's independent spatial mobility in the urban public realm. Childhood, 7(3), 257–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prezza, M. (2007). Children's independent mobility: a review of recent Italian literature. Children, Youth and Environments, 17(4), 293–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prezza, M., Alparone, F. R., Cristallo, C., & Luigi, S. (2005). Parental perception of social risk and of positive potentiality of outdoor autonomy for children: the development of two instruments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(4), 437–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prezza, M., Pilloni, S., Morabito, C., Sersante, C., Alparone, F. R., & Giuliani, M. V. (2001). The influence of psychosocial and environmental factors on children's independent mobility and relationship to peer frequentation. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 11(6), 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rissotto, A., & Tonucci, F. (2002). Freedom of movement and environmental knowledge in elementary school children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22(1), 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, K. A., Morgan, I. G., Ip, J., Kifley, A., Huynh, S., Smith, W., & Mitchell, P. (2008). Outdoor activity reduces the prevalence of myopia in children. Ophthalmology, 115(8), 1279–1285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandseter, E. B. H., & Kennair, L. E. O. (2011). Children’s risky play from an evolutionary perspective: the anti-phobic effects of thrilling experiences. Evolutionary Psychology, 9(2), 257–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, S. (1997). Gender-related strategies in environmental development: effects of anxiety on wayfinding in and representation of a three-dimensional maze. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 17(3), 215–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schug, M. G. (2016). Equal children play best: raising independent Faroese children in the Nordic welfare state. In J. DeLoache, & A. Gottlieb (Eds.), World of babies: imagined childcare guides for nine societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (in press).

  • Segrin, C., Woszidlo, A., Givertz, M., & Montgomery, N. (2013). Parent and child traits associated with overparenting. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32(6), 569–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, I., Choi, J., & Peters, M. (2007). The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: data from 40 countries. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 261–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations Statistics Division (2008). Demographic statistics. Retrieved from http://data.un.org/Data.aspx?d=POPandf=tableCode%3A240

  • Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013). The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 352–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villanueva, K., Giles-Corti, B., Bulsara, M., McCormack, G. R., Timperio, A., et al. (2012). How far do children travel from their homes? Exploring children's activity spaces in their neighborhood. Health and Place, 18(2), 263–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webley, P. (1981). Sex differences in home range and cognitive maps in eight-year-old children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1(4), 293–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank the Spatial Cognition and Navigation Project for their funding (National Science Foundation IBSS 1329091), my local contact Eiler Fagraklett for his assistance with data collection, and Luke Ayers for his technical support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mariah G. Schug.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schug, M.G. Geographical Cues and Developmental Exposure. Hum Nat 27, 68–81 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9245-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9245-4

Keywords

Navigation