Skip to main content
Log in

What Can Cross-Cultural Correlations Teach Us about Human Nature?

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many recent evolutionary psychology and human behavioral ecology studies have tested hypotheses by examining correlations between variables measured at a group level (e.g., state, country, continent). In such analyses, variables collected for each aggregation are often taken to be representative of the individuals present within them, and relationships between such variables are presumed to reflect individual-level processes. There are multiple reasons to exercise caution when doing so, including: (1) the ecological fallacy, whereby relationships observed at the aggregate level do not accurately represent individual-level processes; (2) non-independence of data points, which violates assumptions of the inferential techniques used in null hypothesis testing; and (3) cross-cultural non-equivalence of measurement (differences in construct validity between groups). We provide examples of how each of these gives rise to problems in the context of testing evolutionary hypotheses about human behavior, and we offer some suggestions for future research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anand, S., & Sen, A. (1994). Human Development Index: methodology and measurement. Human Development Occasional Paper No. 2. Oxford University Press on behalf of the United Nations Development Program. Available online at http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/oc12.pdf

  • Anderson, K. G. (2010). Life expectancy and the timing of life history events in developing countries. Human Nature, 21(2), 103–123. doi:10.1007/s12110-010-9087-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apostolou, M. (2010). Sexual selection under parental choice in agropastoral societies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(1), 39–47. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 150–166. doi:10.1177/1088868306294907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO–60: a short measure of the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340–345. doi:10.1080/00223890902935878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2000). The sex ratio as a predictor of cross-national variation in violent crime. Cross Cultural Research, 34(3), 264–282. doi:10.1177/106939710003400304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barber, N. (2003). Paternal investment prospects and cross-national differences in single parenthood. CrossCultural Research, 37(2), 163–177. doi:10.1177/1069397103037002001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, J., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Human social stratification and hypergyny: toward an understanding of male homosexual preference. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 155–163. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2001). Using phylogenetically based comparative methods in anthropology: more questions than answers. Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews, 10(3), 99–111. doi:10.1002/evan.1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Nunn, C. L., & Towner, M. C. (2006). Cultural macroevolution and the transmission of traits. Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews, 15(2), 52–64. doi:10.1002/evan.20088.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Bowles, S., Hertz, T., Bell, A., Beise, J., Clark, G., & Hooper, P. L. (2009). Intergenerational wealth transmission and the dynamics of inequality in small-scale societies. Science, 326(5953), 682–688. doi:10.1126/science.1178336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosker, R. J., Kremers, E. J. J., & Lugthart, E. (1990). School and instruction effects on mathematics achievement. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 1(4), 233–248. doi:10.1080/0924345900010401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, M. S., & McDonald, L. L. (1999). Relating populations to habitats using resource selection functions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 14(7), 268–272. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01593-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, A., & Bradley, M. T. (2012). More voodoo correlations: when average-based measures inflate correlations. Journal of General Psychology, 139(4), 260–272. doi:10.1080/00221309.2012.703711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brand, A., Bradley, M. T., Best, L. A., & Stoica, G. (2010). Multiple trials may yield exaggerated effect size estimates. Journal of General Psychology, 138(1), 1–11. doi:10.1080/00221309.2010.520360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brashares, J. S., Arcese, P., Sam, M. K., Coppolillo, P. B., Sinclair, A. R. E., & Balmford, A. (2004). Bushmeat hunting, wildlife declines, and fish supply in West Africa. Science, 306(5699), 1180–1183. doi:10.1126/science.1102425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. L., Morales, V., & Summers, K. (2009). Home range size and location in relation to reproductive resources in poison frogs (Dendrobatidae): a Monte Carlo approach using GIS data. Animal Behaviour, 77(2), 547–554. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J., Kirchner, M., Bramanti, B., Haak, W., & Thomas, M. G. (2007). Absence of the lactase-persistence-associated allele in early Neolithic Europeans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(10), 3736–3741. doi:10.1073/pnas.0607187104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. M. (2009). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. London, UK: Routledge.

  • Byrne, B. M., & Campbell, T. L. (1999). Cross-cultural comparisons and the presumption of equivalent measurement and theoretical structure. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30(5), 555–574. doi:10.1177/0022022199030005001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cagliani, R., Guerini, F. R., Fumagalli, M., Riva, S., Agliardi, C., Galimberti, D., & Fenoglio, C. (2012). A trans-specific polymorphism in ZC3HAV1 is maintained by long-standing balancing selection and may confer susceptibility to multiple sclerosis. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 29(6), 1599–1613. doi:10.1093/molbev/mss002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cashdan, E., & Steele, M. (2013). Pathogen prevalence, group bias, and collectivism in the standard cross-cultural sample. Human Nature, 24, 59–75. doi:10.1007/s12110-012-9159-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavusgil, S. T., & Das, A. (1997). Methodological issues in empirical cross-cultural research: a survey of the management literature and a framework. Management International Review, 37(1), 71–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, K.-T. (2003). Introduction to geographic information systems. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comaroff, J. L. (1980). The meaning of marriage payments. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, P. (2006). Summary statistics, educational achievement gaps and the ecological fallacy. Oxford Review of Education, 32(02), 235–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & MacCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO FFI): Professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Currie, T. E., & Mace, R. (2012). Analyses do not support the parasite-stress theory of human sociality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 83–85. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., Hasegawa, T., & Mace, R. (2010). Rise and fall of political complexity in island South-East Asia and the Pacific. Nature, 467(7317), 801–804. doi:10.1038/nature09461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, E., Ivar Elstad, J., Hofoss, D., & Martin-Mollard, M. (2006). For whom is income inequality most harmful? A multi-level analysis of income inequality and mortality in Norway. Social Science and Medicine, 63(10), 2562–2574. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.06.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dama, M. S. (2011). Sex ratio at birth and mortality rates are negatively related in humans. PLoS One, 6(8), e23792. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dama, M. S. (2012). Parasite stress predicts offspring sex ratio. PLoS One, 7(9), e46169. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dama, M. S. (2013). Cognitive ability correlates positively with son birth and predicts cross-cultural variation of the offspring sex ratio. Naturwissenschaften, 100(6), 559–569. doi:10.1007/s00114-013-1052-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Crawford, J. R., Welling, L. L. M., & Little, A. C. (2010). The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for masculinized male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1692), 2405–2410. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Crawford, J. R., & Welling, L. L. M. (2011). Further evidence for regional variation in women’s masculinity preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1707), 813–814. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DEFRA. (2011). Family food datasets. Retrieved June 6, 2012, from http://www.defra.gov.uk/statistics/foodfarm/food/familyfood/datasets/.

  • Dow, M. M. (2007). Galton’s Problem as multiple network autocorrelation effects cultural trait transmission and ecological constraint. Cross Cultural Research, 41(4), 336–363. doi:10.1177/1069397107305452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dow, M. M., & Eff, E. A. (2008). Global, regional, and local network autocorrelation in the standard cross-cultural sample. Cross Cultural Research, 42(2), 148–171. doi:10.1177/1069397107311186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(6), 541–546. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, D. G. (1994). Patriarchy and wife assault: the ecological fallacy. Violence and Victims, 9(2), 167–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eff, E. A. (2004). Does Mr. Galton still have a problem? Autocorrelation in the standard cross-cultural sample. World Cultures, 15(2), 153–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eggan, F. (1954). Social anthropology and the method of controlled comparison. American Anthropologist, 56(5), 743–763. doi:10.1525/aa.1954.56.5.02a00020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eppig, C., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2010). Parasite prevalence and the worldwide distribution of cognitive ability. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1701), 3801–3808. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0973.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eppig, C., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2011). Parasite prevalence and the distribution of intelligence among the states of the USA. Intelligence, 39(3), 155–160. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2011.02.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: the cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(2), 65–79. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000021.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1640), 1279–1285. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firebaugh, G. (1978). A rule for inferring individual-level relationships from aggregate data. American Sociological Review, 43(4), 557–572. doi:10.2307/2094779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankenhuis, W. E., & Panchanathan, K. (2011). Balancing sampling and specialization: an adaptationist model of incremental development. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1724), 3558–3565. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankenhuis, W. E., Panchanathan, K., & Clark Barrett, H. (2013). Bridging developmental systems theory and evolutionary psychology using dynamic optimization. Developmental Science, 16(4), 584–98. doi:10.1111/desc.12053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., Shor, B., Bafumi, J., & Park, D. (2007). Rich state, poor state, red state, blue state: what’s the matter with Connecticut? Quarterly Journal of Political Science, 2(4), 345–367. doi:10.1561/100.00006026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golding, J., Pembrey, M., & Jones, R. (2001). ALSPAC–the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology, 15(1), 74–87. doi:10.1046/j.1365-3016.2001.00325.x.

  • Gravelle, H. (1998). How much of the relation between population mortality and unequal distribution of income is a statistical artefact? British Medical Journal, 316(7128), 382–385. doi:10.1136/bmj.316.7128.382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: a life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1015–1026. doi:10.1037/a0022403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, J., & Hruschka, D. (2013). Fast life histories, not pathogens, account for state-level variation in homicide, child maltreatment, and family ties in the U.S. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(2), 118–124. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammel, E. A. (1980). The comparative method in anthropological perspective. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 22(2), 145–155. doi:10.2307/178402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61(1), 139–151. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. H., Read, A. F., & Nee, S. (1995). Why ecologists need to be phylogenetically challenged. Journal of Ecology, 83(3), 535–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1994). On life-history evolution. Current Anthropology, 35(1), 39–41. doi:10.2307/2744136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., & Greenholtz, J. (2002). What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 903–918. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Buchtel, E. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). What do cross-national comparisons of personality traits tell us? Psychological Science, 19(4), 309–313. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02085.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herkenrath, M. (2002). Quantitative cross-national analysis as a research tool in the sociology of developing countries: a critical examination. Current Sociology, 50(4), 517–530. doi:10.1177/0011392102050004003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. R., Walker, R. S., Božičević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., & Wood, B. (2011). Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331(6022), 1286–1289. doi:10.1126/science.1199071.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holý, L. (1987). Comparative anthropology. (L. Holý, Ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.

  • Howell, D. C. (2010). Fundamental statistics for the behavioral sciences. Belmont: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: techniques and applications. London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1985). Measurement in cross-cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 16(2), 131–152. doi:10.1177/0022002185016002001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huisman, M., Oldehinkel, A. J., De Winter, A., Minderaa, R. B., De Bildt, A., Huizink, A. C., & Ormel, J. (2008). Cohort profile: the Dutch “TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey” (TRAILS). International Journal of Epidemiology, 37(6), 1227–1235. doi:10.1093/ije/dym273.

  • John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: theory and research (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanazawa, S. (2006). IQ and the wealth of states. Intelligence, 34(6), 593–600. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kievit, R. A., Frankenhuis, W. E., Waldorp, L. J., & Borsboom, D. (2013). Simpson’s Paradox in psychological science: a practical guide. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 513. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, G., Murray, C. J. L., Salomon, J. A., & Tandon, A. (2003). Enhancing the validity and cross-cultural comparability of measurement in survey research. The American Political Science Review, 97(4), 567–583. doi:10.1017/S000305540400108X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, D. J., & Nesse, R. M. (2006). An evolutionary life-history framework for understanding sex differences in human mortality rates. Human Nature, 17(1), 74–97. doi:10.1007/s12110-006-1021-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, D. J., & Schlemmer, E. (2009). When men are scarce, good men are even harder to find: life history, the sex ratio, and the proportion of men married. Journal of Social Evolutionary and Cultural Psychology, 3(2), 93–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 329–358. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., Apicella, C. L., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Preferences for symmetry in human faces in two cultures: data from the UK and the Hadza, an isolated group of hunter-gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1629), 3113–3117. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubinski, D., & Humphreys, L. G. (1996). Seeing the forest from the trees: when predicting the behavior or status of groups, correlate means. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2(2), 363. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.2.2.363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum, R. C., & Austin, J. T. (2000). Applications of structural equation modeling in psychological research. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 201–226. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., & Pagel, M. (1994). The comparative method in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 35(5), 549–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R., Jordan, F., & Holden, C. (2003). Testing evolutionary hypotheses about human biological adaptation using cross-cultural comparison. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology. Part A, Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 136(1), 85–94. doi:10.1016/S1095-6433(03)00019-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2003). The mating system of foragers in the standard cross-cultural sample. Cross Cultural Research, 37(3), 282–306. doi:10.1177/1069397103254008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W., Berbesque, J. C., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., & Tracer, D. (2008). More “altruistic” punishment in larger societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1634), 587–592. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W., Berbesque, J. C., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Gurven, M., & Tracer, D. (2011). The “spiteful” origins of human cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1715), 2159–2164. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCormick, C. M. (2013). Watch where and how you stick pins when playing with voodoo correlations. Journal of General Psychology, 140(1), 82–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch, C. E., & Neuhaus, J. M. (2001). Generalized linear mixed models. New York: Wiley Online Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menzel, H. (1950). Comment on Robinson’s “Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals.” American Sociological Review, 15, 674.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mgawe, P., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Caro, T., Martin, A., & Kiffner, C. (2012). Factors affecting bushmeat consumption in the Katavi-Rukwa ecosystem of Tanzania. Tropical Conservation Science, 5(4), 446–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. H. (1994). Putting anthropology back together again: the ethnogenetic critique of cladistic theory. American Anthropologist, 96(4), 925–948. doi:10.2307/682452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, F. R., Coetzee, V., Contreras-Garduño, J., Debruine, L. M., Kleisner, K., Krams, I., … Suzuki, T. N. (2013). Cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for cues to sex- and stress-hormones in the male face. Biology Letters, 9(3). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0050.

  • Murdock, G. P., & White, D. R. (1969). Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology, 8(4), 329–369. doi:10.2307/3772907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naroll, R. (1965). Galton’s Problem: the logic of cross-cultural analysis. Social Research, 32(4), 428–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nasif, E. G., Al-Daeaj, H., Ebrahimi, B., & Thibodeaux, M. S. (1991). Methodological problems in cross-cultural research: an updated review. Management International Review, 31(1), 79–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Child Development Study. (n.d.). National Child Development Study. Retrieved June 06, 2012, from http://www.esds.ac.uk/longitudinal/access/ncds/l33004.asp.

  • Nee, S., Read, A. F., Greenwood, J. J. D., & Harvey, P. H. (1991). The relationship between abundance and body size in British birds. Nature, 351(6324), 312–313. doi:10.1038/351312a0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nee, S., Read, A. F., & Harvey, P. H. (1996). Why phylogenies are necessary for comparative analysis. In E. P. Martins (Ed.), Phylogenies and the comparative method in animal behavior (pp. 399–411). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nettle, D. (2009). Ecological influences on human behavioural diversity: a review of recent findings. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 24(11), 618–624. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.05.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piantadosi, S., Byar, D. P., & Green, S. B. (1988). The ecological fallacy. American Journal of Epidemiology, 127(5), 893–904.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plewis, I., Ketende, S., & Millennium Cohort Study. (2006). Millennium Cohort Study. Technical Report on Responses. Retrieved from http://www.esds.ac.uk/longitudinal/access/mcs/l33359.asp.

  • Poortinga, Y. H. (1989). Equivalence of cross-cultural data: an overview of basic issues. International Journal of Psychology, 24(1), 737–756. doi:10.1080/00207598908247842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 1298–1301. doi:10.1126/science.1170165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, R., & Quinlan, M. (2008). Human lactation, pair-bonds, and alloparents. Human Nature, 19(1), 87–102. doi:10.1007/s12110-007-9026-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. (2000). Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 1–48. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rehkopf, D. H., & Buka, S. L. (2006). The association between suicide and the socio-economic characteristics of geographical areas: a systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 36(02), 145–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, W. S. (1950). Ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals. American Sociological Review, 15(3), 351–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, M. H., & Homer, E. (1976). Galton’s Problem in cross-national research. World Politics, 29(1), 1–28. doi:10.2307/2010045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, S. P., Lurz, P. W. W., Fuller, R., & Garson, P. J. (1997). Modelling the distribution of the red and grey squirrel at the landscape scale: a combined GIS and population dynamics approach. Journal of Applied Ecology, 34(5), 1137–1154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaller, M., & Murray, D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 212–221. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheiner, S. M., Cox, S. B., Willig, M., Mittelbach, G. G., Osenberg, C., & Kaspari, M. (2000). Species richness, species-area curves and Simpson’s Paradox. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2(6), 791–802. doi:10.1152/ajpendo.00279.2010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 168–182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. (1994). The fallacy of the ecological fallacy: the potential misuse of a concept and the consequences. American Journal of Public Health, 84(5), 819–824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheppard, L. (2003). Insights on bias and information in group‐level studies. Biostatistics, 4(2), 265–278. doi:10.1093/biostatistics/4.2.265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shrira, I., Wisman, A., & Webster, G. D. (2013). Guns, germs, and stealing: exploring the link between infectious disease and crime. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(1), 270–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, E. H. (1951). The interpretation of interaction in contingency tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 13(2), 238–241. doi:10.1038/168063d0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephen, I. D., Scott, I. M. L., Coetzee, V., Pound, N., Perrett, D. I., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men’s faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(4), 260–267. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Subramanian, S. V., Jones, K., Kaddour, A., & Krieger, N. (2009). Revisiting Robinson: the perils of individualistic and ecologic fallacy. International Journal of Epidemiology, 38(2), 342–360. doi:10.1093/ije/dyn359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. (2007). National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, 1997. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR). doi:10.3886/ICPSR03959.v2.

  • Te Grotenhuis, M., Eisinga, R., & Subramanian, S. V. (2011). Robinson’s ecological correlations and the behavior of individuals: methodological corrections. International Journal of Epidemiology, 40(4), 1123–1125. doi:10.1093/ije/dyn357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terracciano, A., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Adam, N., Adamovova, L., Ahn, C., Ahn, H., & Angleitner, A. (2005). National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 cultures. Science, 310(5745), 96–100. doi:10.1126/science.1117199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, F., Daoust, S. P., Elguero, E., & Raymond, M. (2013). Do mothers from rich and well-nourished countries bear more sons? Journal of Evolutionary Medicine, 1, 1–11. doi:10.4303/jem/235568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1939). On the fallacy of imputing the correlations found for groups to the individuals or smaller groups composing them. American Journal of Psychology, 52(1), 122–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2011). Parasite stress promotes homicide and child maltreatment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 3466–3477. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tovée, M. J., Swami, V., Furnham, A., & Mangalparsad, R. (2006). Changing perceptions of attractiveness as observers are exposed to a different culture. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(6), 443–456. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towner, M. C., Grote, M. N., Venti, J., & Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2012). Cultural macroevolution on neighbor graphs. Human Nature, 23(3), 283–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turchin, P., Currie, T. E., Turner, E. A. L., & Gavrilets, S. (2013). War, space, and the evolution of Old World complex societies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(41), 16384–16389. doi:10.1073/pnas.1308825110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tybur, J. M., & De Vries, R. E. (2013). Disgust sensitivity and the HEXACO model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(6), 660–665. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tybur, J. M., Bryan, A. D., Lieberman, D., Caldwell Hooper, A. E., & Merriman, L. A. (2011). Sex differences and sex similarities in disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 343–348. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United Nations Development Programme (UNDP). (2011). Human Development Index (HDI) data. http://hdr.undp.org/en/statistics/hdi. Accessed November 2011.

  • Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (2000). Methodological issues in psychological research on culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31(1), 33–51. doi:10.1177/0022022100031001004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Tanzer, N. K. (2004). Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural assessment: an overview. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European Review of Applied Psychology, 54(2), 119–135. doi:10.1016/j.erap.2003.12.004.

  • Vleesbarometer. (2011). Thuisverbruik (Home Consumption). Retrieved June 06, 2012, from http://www.vlam.be/marketinformationdocument/files/Thuisverbruik2010.pdf.

  • Von Korff, M., Koepsell, T., Curry, S., & Diehr, P. (1992). Multi-level analysis in epidemiologic research on health behaviors and outcomes. American Journal of Epidemiology, 135(10), 1077–1082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., & Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(3), 274–290. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, L. A., & Gotway, C. A. (2004). Applied spatial statistics for public health data. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, E. A. (1993). Comparative cognition: beginning the second century of the study of animal intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 211–228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R manual: wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, J. C. K. (2012). Sexual dimorphism in body composition across human populations: associations with climate and proxies for short- and long-term energy supply. American Journal of Human Biology, 24(4), 411–419. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, D. R., Betzig, L., Mulder, M. B., Chick, G., Hartung, J., Irons, W., & Spencer, P. (1988). Rethinking polygyny: co-wives, codes, and cultural systems [and comments and reply]. Current Anthropology, 29(4), 529–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkie, D. S., Starkey, M., Abernethy, K., Effa, E. N., Telfer, P., & Godoy, R. (2005). Role of prices and wealth in consumer demand for bushmeat in Gabon, Central Africa. Conservation Biology, 19(1), 268–274. doi:10.1111/j.1523-1739.2005.00372.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkin, T. A., Garant, D., Gosler, A. G., & Sheldon, B. C. (2006). Density effects on life‐history traits in a wild population of the great tit Parus major: analyses of long‐term data with GIS techniques. Journal of Animal Ecology, 75(2), 604–615. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2006.01078.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkleby, M., Cubbin, C., & Ahn, D. (2006). Effect of cross-level interaction between individual and neighborhood socioeconomic status on adult mortality rates. American Journal of Public Health, 96(12), 2145–2153. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2004.060970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, H., Chong, H., & Auffhammer, M. (2011). Classification, detection and consequences of data error: evidence from the human development index. The Economic Journal, 121(553), 843–847. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0297.2010.02408.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yip, P. S. F., & Liu, K. Y. (2006). The ecological fallacy and the gender ratio of suicide in China. British Journal of Psychiatry, 189(5), 465–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • York, R., & Gossard, M. H. (2004). Cross-national meat and fish consumption: exploring the effects of modernization and ecological context. Ecological Economics, 48(3), 293–302. doi:10.1016/j.ecolecon.2003.10.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Daniel Nettle, two anonymous reviewers, and Monique Borgerhoff Mulder for providing us with helpful and extensive comments to improve earlier versions of this manuscript. Thomas Pollet is supported by The Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (Veni, 451.10.032)

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas V. Pollet.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pollet, T.V., Tybur, J.M., Frankenhuis, W.E. et al. What Can Cross-Cultural Correlations Teach Us about Human Nature?. Hum Nat 25, 410–429 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9206-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-014-9206-3

Keywords

Navigation