Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Paradigms extended: how to integrate behaviorism, constructivism, knowledge domain, and learner mastery in instructional design

  • Development Article
  • Published:
Educational technology research and development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The pedagogical paradigms of Direct instruction (behaviorism/objectivism) and Constructivism are often seen as opposing paradigms at the ends of an instructional design continuum. Unfortunately, this view makes the two approaches mutually exclusive. Designers must use the one at the expense of the other. A previous study proposed that the two approaches should be integrated along two axes, producing a matrix of four quadrants of learning, called “injection”, “immersion”, “construction” and “integration”. This model maps directly onto the Cynefin framework of knowledge management that contains four knowledge domains, “known”, “chaos”, “complex” and “knowable”. The combined models allow for a mix of direct, or constructivist teaching and learning. The problem is that the model addresses only two elements of the pedagogical triangle of teacher, content, and learner. It integrates knowledge domains (learning material) with direct and generative approaches to teaching but is silent on the needs of learners. In this paper we propose an extension of the model to add learner mastery to the domains of knowledge, and teaching and learning methods to extend the current decision matrix by adding the learning curve. We consider the metrics for deriving a learning curve, and then fit the required- or expected level of competency and the learner’s level of general competency to the learning curve. We then map the curve to the four quadrants to establish when a particular quadrant is appropriate at a specific phase of the learning curve. This improved model may assist teachers, (lecturers, instructional designers) and learners to select appropriate pedagogical paradigms and methods, based on the domain of knowledge and learners’ mastery of learning outcomes. Finally, we propose a sequential approach to selecting learning paradigms, based on how much of the material has been mastered.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Actes, P., Murray, S., Cormier, D., Stewart, B., Pearson, J., & Seager, S. (2010). Atlantic Universities ’ Teaching Showcase L ’ Expo - Engseignement des Universités de l ’ Atlantique Edited By / Sous la direction de Plenaries Papers. XIV. https://www.atlanticuniversities.ca/system/files/documents/AAUTeachingShowcaseProceedings/showcaseproceedings2010.pdf#page=24

  • Ahmad, S., Sultana, N., & Jamil, S. (2020). Behaviorism vs constructivism: A paradigm shift from traditional to alternative assessment techniques. Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 7(2), 19–33 https://www.jallr.com.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aylward, R. C. (2012). The three phases of learning. ICERI 2012, Phnom Penh, 47(7), 32–36. https://doi.org/10.7763/IPEDR

  • Aylward, R. C. (2018). Learning curves: Insights and conflicts towards more effective learning. ICERI2018 Proceedings, 1935–1943.

  • Aylward, R. C., Van Wyk, B. J., & Hamam, Y. (2019). Rope-weaver’s principles: Towards more effective learning. SAIEE Africa Research Journal, 110(4), 266. https://doi.org/10.23919/saiee.2019.8864146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahari, S. F. (2012). Qualitative versus quantitative research strategies: contrasting epistemological and ontological assumptions. Jurnal Teknologi, 52(1), 17–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Gal, G., Katorza, L., Weiss, E. I., & Ziv, A. (2017). Testing motor learning curves among dental students. Journal of Dental Education, 81(10), 1171–1178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. S. (1986). Automaticity: “The hands and feet of genius". Educational Leadership, 43(5), 70–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burch, N. (1970). Four stages of competency. Gordon Training International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. (1995). Analysis in Instructional Design. Big Dog & Little Dog’s Performance Juxtaposition. https://www.nwlink.com/~donclark/hrd/sat2.html

  • Council of Europe. (n.d.). Common reference levels. Common European Framework of Reference for Languages. Retrieved October 7, 2021 from https://www.coe.int/en/web/common-european-framework-reference-languages/table-1-cefr-3.3-common-reference-levels-global-scale

  • Cronjé, J. C. (2000). Paradigms lost: Towards integrating objectivism and constructivism. ITForum. http://itforum.coe.uga.edu/paper48/paper48.html

  • Cronjé, J. C. (2006). Paradigms regained: Toward integrating objectivism and constructivism in instructional design and the learning sciences. Educational Technology Research and Development, 54(4), 387–416. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-006-9605-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronjé, J. C. (2020). Towards a new definition of blended learning. Electronic Journal of E-Learning, 18(2), 114–121. https://doi.org/10.34190/EJEL.20.18.2.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ditz, H. M., & Nieder, A. (1827). Numerosity representations in crows obey the Weber-Fechner law. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283, 266. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.0083.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Souza Neves, I. L. A., & Neves, F. O. (2021). Educational public policies on English Language Teaching in Brazil Based on Neuroscience and Psychology of Human Development. American Scientific Research Journal for Engineering, Technology, and Sciences (ASRJETS), 76(1), 12–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, S. E., & Dreyfus, H. L. (1980). A five-stage model of the mental activities involved in directed skill acquisition. California University Berkeley Operations Research Center

  • Elander, K. R., & Cronjé, J. C. (2016). Paradigms revisited: A quantitative investigation into a model to integrate objectivism and constructivism in instructional design. Educational Technology Research and Development, 64(3), 389–405. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-016-9424-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ertmer, P. A., & Newby, T. J. (2013). Behaviorism, cognitivism, constructivism: Comparing critical features from an instructional design perspective. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 26(2), 43–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G. T. (1907). Elemente der Psychophysik I u. II. In Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel

  • Fechner, G. T. (2012). Elemente der psychophysik. Рипол Классик

  • Gagné, R. M. (1985). The conditions of learning and theory of instruction. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, R. M., & Briggs, L. J. (1974). Principles of instructional design. Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gredler, M. E. (1992). Learning and instruction: Theory into practice. Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grene, M. (1971). Knowing and being: essays by Michael Polanyi

  • Hassad, R. A. (2011). Constructivist and behaviorist approaches: Development and initial evaluation of a teaching practice scale for introductory statistics at the college level. Numeracy: Advancing Education in Quantitative Literacy, 4(2), 1–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatala, R., Gutman, J., Lineberry, M., Triola, M., & Pusic, M. (2019). How well is each learner learning? Validity investigation of a learning curve-based assessment approach for ECG interpretation. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 24(1), 45–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Houssaye, J. (1988). Le triangle pédagogique. Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyslop-Margison, E. J., & Strobel, J. (2007). Constructivism and education: Misunderstandings and pedagogical implications. The Teacher Educator, 43(1), 72–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H. (1991). Objectivism versus constructivism: Do we need a new philosophical paradigm? Educational Technology Research and Development, 39(3), 5–14

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khan, A. S., & Nawaz, A. (2010). Digital literacy: the criteria for being educated in information society. Global Journal of Computer Science and Technology, 10, 10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurtz, C. F., & Snowden, D. J. (2003). The new dynamics of strategy: Sense-making in a complex and complicated world. IBM Systems Journal, 42(3), 462–483. http://vdc.edu.au/wp-content/uploads/2018/02/Sense-making-in-a-complex-and-complicated-world.pdf

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meek, E. L. (2017). Contact with reality: Michael Polanyi’s realism and why it matters. Wipf and Stock.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pande, M., & Bharathi, S. V. (2020). Theoretical foundations of design thinking: A constructivism learning approach to design thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 36, 100637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papert, S. (1993). The children’s machine: Rethinking school in the age of the computer. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, C. J., Gali, V. S., Patel, D. V., & Parmar, R. D. (2011). The effects of information and communication technologies (ICTs) on higher education: From objectivism to social constructivism. International Journal of Vocational and Technical Education, 3(5), 113–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Pusic, M., Pecaric, M., & Boutis, K. (2011). How much practice is enough? Using learning curves to assess the deliberate practice of radiograph interpretation. Academic Medicine, 86(6), 731–736

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L. P. (1992). Computer-based microworlds: A bridge between constructivism and direct instruction. Educational Technology Research and Development, 40(1), 93–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schein, E. H. (1996). Kurt Lewin’s change theory in the field and in the classroom: Notes toward a model of managed learning. Systems Practice, 9(1), 27–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shabo, A. (1997). Integrating constructionism and instructionism in educational hypermedia programs. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 17(3), 231–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. (1938). The behavior of organisms. In Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York

  • Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64(3), 153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, S. S. (1961). To honor Fechner and repeal his law. Science, 133(3446), 80–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L. (1919). The learning curve equation. Psychological Monographs, 26(3), 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrasidas, C. (2000). Constructivism versus objectivism: Implications for interaction, course design, and evaluation in distance education. International Journal of Educational Telecommunications, 6(4), 339–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). In M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, E. H. (1834). De pulsu, resorptione, auditu et tactu: annotationes anatomicae et physiologicae, auctore. prostat apud CF Koehler

  • Zlatkova-Doncheva, K. (2020). Constructivism and behaviorism in teaching children with intellectual disabilities

  • Zorn, C., Bauer, E., Feffer, M. L., Moerschel, E., Bierry, G., Choquet, P., & Dillenseger, J. P. (2020). Building and exploitation of learning curves to train radiographer students in X-ray CT image postprocessing. Journal of Medical Imaging and Radiation Sciences, 51(1), 173–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johannes C. Cronjé.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest associated with this research.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

7. Appendix: The mathematics

7. Appendix: The mathematics

This section describes the logic behind the development of the learning curve and is presented as additional information.

External view of learning process

Central to the first phase was the development of tools to assist in the measurement and collection of data. As it is currently nearly impossible to directly measure the changes that occur in the brain during learning, alternative measures need to be developed. It is a well observed fact that the brain responds perceivably different to different ‘strengths’ of stimuli (Weber, 1834). According to the works of Weber and Fechner described as the Weber-Fechner Law (Fechner, 1907, 2012),

$$P=K\log I$$

where \(P\) is the perceived magnitude, \(K\) is Weber’s constant and \(I\), the stimulus impulse or strength.

Based on work of Weber and Fechner, it was decided to measure the brain’s response in how it well it reacts when confronted with solving tasks involving the stimulus, the new knowledge component (both knowledge and skill). Four assignments, tasks involving “separable” knowledge components, were selected from the course, Examples 1–4, and the students’ performance on these tasks measured. This involved Time-on-Task (ToT) measurements to reach a preset proficiency. The Time-on-Task measures were measured for each student individually for the four different knowledge components (Examples 1–4): two tasks that are closely related and may be presented in any order (Example 1 and 2) at the start of the course; two tasks that need to be presented in order (Example 3 and 4) presented in the latter half of the course.

The total ToT measures were taken over specified periods; for the delivery of the new content, Tuition phase (T0–T1) the time spent to complete the delivery of theoretical components and Demonstration phase (T1–T2) the time spent to complete the demonstrations of practical applications of theory, the Reproduction phase (T2–T3) the time spent by the students before they are able to demonstrate their ability to duplicate results obtained in the demonstration phase, the Application phase (T3–T4) the time spent by the students before they are able to demonstrate their ability to apply the new knowledge component, and finally, the students enter into the Expert phase at time T4, the phase where students are ready to be assessed for mastery of the knowledge component under review. The Time-on-Task (ToT) measures, T0–T1–T2–T3–T4, are presented as colored bars at the bottom of for the different phases of teaching and learning.

The ToT measures were taken for each student individually and averaged for all the students in the study. summarizes the averaged ToT measures for a combination of different time intervals for the four tasks. It is represented as the time that elapsed since the start or introduction (T0) of the specific knowledge component and the set outcome level is reached (T2, T3 or T4), as an example the Tuition and Demonstration phase (T0—T2), for each of the Examples 1–4. From this data, it was observed that there was a plateau in the learning curve (Aylward, 2018) (Table 5).

Table 5 Time-on-Task measures

Using regression and the data from the learning curve can be constructed using two interchangeable models:

Model 1:

$${\text{P}}_{{1}} (t) = \alpha (1 - e^{\beta t} ) + (1 - \alpha )\left( {\frac{{e^{(mt + c)} }}{{1 + e^{{\left( {mt + c} \right)}} }}} \right)\;with\;0 < \alpha ,\;\beta < 1.$$

Model 2:

$${\text{P}}_{2} (t) = \alpha (1 - e^{ - \beta t} ) + \left( {1 - \alpha } \right)(1 - e^{{ - \eta (e^{\gamma t - 1} )}} )\;with\;0 < \alpha ,\beta ,\gamma ,\eta < 1$$

where \(\alpha\) represents the level of the plateau. The plateau represents the fundamental knowledge needed to understand the knowledge component, its purpose and function. This is a percentage of the total knowledge to be gained for the whole knowledge component to be mastered, usually \(\alpha =0.1\) of the total (1) but could also be \(\alpha =1\) for the exponential learning curve. β represents the learning rate for the first phase and γ the learning rate for the second phase. \(\eta\) represents the time when the student reaches the Expert phase at time T4 (Figs. 15, 16, 17). The formulas produce similar results and describe a whole family of curves that models different rates of progression for mastery of knowledge components. This includes the exponential learning curve (Top left–black curve) through to the newly defined learning curve with the plateau (Bottom right–red curve). The learner’s rate of progression (R) is a function of the learner/student/trainee themselves (l), the environment (e) and the current knowledge-skill level (k)–R is thus a function the current belief and value system within a set environment: \(R=f\left(l,e,k\right).\) This relates back to the driving and opposing forces to learning presented in Fig. 3 (Table 5).

Fig. 15
figure 15

External view of learning curve (Extened from Aylward et al. 2019)

The different teaching and learning intervals, T0–T1–T2–T3–T4, have been introduced above and the measured results are presented in a graphical format. Both graphs, black and red, are derived from either model equations. Black represents the case where the material is apparent, easy, and thus quickly mastered without going through the plateau phase–the standard exponential learning curve. The red graph represents learning that first goes through the plateau phase, slow progress or learning–I don’t know what I’m doing or even supposed to do phase, before rapidly mastering the material, the ah-ha I’ve got it phase. There exist a whole family of curves ranging between these two curves. These curves describe the external view of learning progression.

Internal view of learning process

In consolidating the various levels of mastery and the learning curve as discussed above Aylward proposes an eight-level taxonomy of mastery that can be fitted to the learning curve (Table 3). This is an expansion of the levels defined and measured above to reflect the difference between theoretical practice and real-world practical application, thus adding three levels to the proposed 5 level structure of Dreyfus & Dreyfus (1980, p. 50). The proposed levels relate problem difficulty (well defined, broadly defined and ill defined), competence level and confidence level as compiled in. This level descriptor resonates with the Council of Europe’s (Council of Europe, n.d.) which proposes a six-level description of mastery of language, ranging from basic user, through independent user to proficient user. Each of the three categories has an upper and lower range. Ours, however includes a level 0 (unaware) and a level 7, which goes beyond proficient, bringing it to eight.

The eight stages as presented in are now plotted onto learning curve as is shown in for the various stages of mastery.

Fig. 16
figure 16

Knowledge-skill level

It was further observed by Aylward et al. (2019) that stimuli in close proximity to subsidiary knowledge, Fig. 7, had a much shorter interval, Example 2 in, than larger stimuli, more difficult concepts, Examples 3 & 4. During the evaluation of mastery of all knowledge components at the end of the semester it was found that many students inexplicably struggled, even with knowledge components already mastered. To explain this observation wider literature study was required to build a robust measure.

Stevens expanded on the works of Weber and Fechner to derive a log-based scale (Stevens, 1957, 1961):

$$\log b-\log a=\log c -\log b=\log d-\log c$$
Fig. 17
figure 17

Logarithmic interval scale

It was observed, (Aylward, 2018) that the total time needed to master the knowledge components also showed a logarithmic relation. Following the Weber-Fechner Law by taking the log of the ToT measures of, we arrive at a linear measure for the perceived difficulty, forming the base measure for the perceived difficulty, as proposed by Steven,and used by Aylward to measure the perceived difficulty experienced by students when confronted by learning stimuli.

Fig. 18
figure 18

Log scale calculations

Now we can make statements such as: one knowledge component is twice or half as difficult when compared to another knowledge component. Similar results were also found in the way the brain responds to stimuli in other species (“Numerosity Representations in Crows Obey the Weber-Fechner Law,” 2006) leading the researchers to believe that the brain is hardwired in the way it responds to stimuli when confronted with different levels of stimuli. Known knowledge can be masked by ‘large’ stimuli, unfamiliar question or style of question, during evaluation of knowledge or skill obtained.

Using the measures developed and the literature surveyed a new internal view of the learning process was proposed by Aylward (2018). The Dreyfus brothers defined five stages (levels) of learning: novice, advanced beginner, Competent performer, proficiency and expertise (Dreyfus & Dreyfus, 1980, p. 50). Based on the works of Dreyfus & Dreyfus, Aylward et al. expanded the different stages into levels of mastery including three additional levels in order to distinguish between purely theoretical and application-oriented mastery. These and other learning competency level descriptors are overlaid on the learning curve. In Fig. 6 the four internal phases of learning are shown, including the mastery levels overlay. We call these four phases phases: Introduction–first contact with subject matter; Germination–development of fundamental understanding through observation and duplication—behaviorism (objectivism); Digest–integration of new knowledge into believe system—scaffolding; Regenerate—generation of new recipes, procedures and algorithms. This is true for both the mastery of a single knowledge component or the mastery of a wider field knowledge, i.e. ranging from preschool through to the highest tertiary level–spanning from simple mathematical concepts to advanced mathematics (Figs. 18, 19).

Fig. 19
figure 19

Internal view of the learning process

In this section it was shown how a learning participant ‘moves’ between different levels of learning–the internal view of learning progression. The study points to the brain being hardwired in the way it responds to stimuli. If one understands this process one can develop measures to support learning in a more predictable way.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Aylward, R.C., Cronjé, J.C. Paradigms extended: how to integrate behaviorism, constructivism, knowledge domain, and learner mastery in instructional design. Education Tech Research Dev 70, 503–529 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-022-10089-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-022-10089-w

Keywords

Navigation