Skip to main content
Log in

Facilitating Automation in Sentence Processing: The Emergence of Topic and Presupposition in Human Communication

  • Published:
Topoi Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Human attention is limited in its capacity and duration. In language, this is manifested in many ways, but more conspicuously in the strategies by which information is distributed in utterances, that is, their information structures. We contend that the pragmatic categories of Topic and Presupposition precisely meet the necessity to modulate attentional resources on sentence contents, and they do this by “directing” certain contents to automatic and others to controlled processing mechanisms. We discuss experimental findings suggesting that presupposed or topicalized information correlates with automatic processing, and we suggest that this association grounded for the emergence of topic and presupposition units in human communication. We also put forth the processing automaticity induced by these units as the (possible) rationale behind their persuasiveness in some specific contexts of language use (e.g. political discourse and advertising).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the rest of the present work, we will sometimes refer to the STM store as Working Memory (WM), although this latter identifies a particularly dynamic component of the Short-Term Memory store.

  2. It must be acknowledged that the distinction between automatic and controlled processes is one of the most contentious ones in the current literature. Indeed, previous experimental research (Hahne and Friederici 1999) on this double-modality processing has revealed that precise electrophysiological components seem to correlate to both modalities with qualitatively different kinds of effort required of the processor (cf. Sect. 5 on this issue). However, for the purpose of our discussion we will comply with Shiffrin & Schneider’s definition quoted above.

  3. Other test sentences always revolved around world knowledge contents (see Erickson and Mattson 1981, for other examples.

  4. “Garden path” effects are usually observed in syntax, when the first part of an utterance suggests a certain interpretation (e.g. The horse raced past the hedge…), which eventually proves wrong when the rest of the utterance is processed (the horse raced past the hedge had no rider), causing the “going back” to a previous position to take a different path of interpretation. Still, nothing prevents similar reanalysis phenomena to arise also for the informational structure of utterances.

  5. In other models, implicit contents triggered by partial quantification are described as conversational implicatures.

  6. This is the beginning of a broadcasted interview (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GlVO87Qdm-M).

References

  • Arbib MA (2002) The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language. In: Chrystopher N, Kerstin D (eds) Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 229–280

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib MA (2003) The evolving mirror system: a neural basis for language readiness. In: Christiansen MH, Kirby S (eds) Language evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton D (2000) How protolanguage became language. In: Knight C, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford J (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origin of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 264–284

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Birch S, Rayner K (1997) Linguistic focus affects eye movements during reading. Mem Cognit 25(5):653–660

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bredart S, Modolo K (1988) Moses strikes again: focalization effect on a semantic illusion. Acta Psychol 67(2):135–144

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent D (1958) Perception and communication. Pergamon Press, London

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bundesen C, Habekost T (2008) Principles of visual attention: linking mind and brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Burkhardt P (2007) The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. Neuroreport 18(17):1851–1854. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f1a999

  • Carreiras M, Clifton C (2004) The on-line study of sentence comprehension. Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe W (1987) Cognitive constraints on information flow. In: Tomlin R (ed) Coherence and grounding in discourse. Benjamins, Philadelphia, pp 21–51

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chafe W (1992) Information flow in speaking and writing. In: Downing P, Lima SD, Noonan M (eds) The linguistics of literacy. Benjamins, Philadelphia, pp 17–29

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chafe W (1994) Discourse, consciousness, and time. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler P, Sweller J (1992) The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction. Brit J Educ Psychol 62:233–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan N, Elliott ME, Saults SJ et al (2005) On the capacity of attention: its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cogn Psychol 51(1):42–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cresti E (2000) Corpus di italiano parlato. Accademia della Crusca, Firenze

    Google Scholar 

  • Deubel H, Schneider WX (2004) Attentional selection in sequential manual movements, movements around an obstacle and in grasping. In: Humphries GW, Riddoch MJ (eds) Attention in action. Psychology Press, Hove, pp 61–91

    Google Scholar 

  • Domaneschi F, Carrea E, Penco C, Greco A (2014) The cognitive load of presupposition triggers: mandatory and optional repairs in presupposition failure. Lang Cogn Neurosci 29(1):136–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domaneschi F, Carrea E, Penco C, Greco A (2016) Selecting presuppositions in conditional clauses. Results from a psycholinguistic experiment. Front Psychol 6:2026. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ducrot O (1972) Dire et ne pas dire. Hermann, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson TD, Mattson ME (1981) From words to meaning: a semantic illusion. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 20(5):540–551

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauker C (1998) What is a context of utterance? Philos Stud 91:149–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gauker C (2002) Words without meaning. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Givón T (1975) Focus and the scope of assertion: some bantu evidence. Stud Afr Ling 6:185–205

    Google Scholar 

  • Givón T (1991) Some substantive issues concerning verb serialization: grammatical versus cognitive packaging. In: Lefebvre C (ed) Serial verbs: grammatical, comparative, and cognitive approaches. John Benjamins, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Givón T (2002) Bio-linguistics. John Benjamins, Philadelphia

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hahne A, Friederici AD (1999) Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: early automatic and late controlled processes. J Cogn Neurosci 11(2):194–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hornby PA (1974) Surface structure and presupposition. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 13(5):530–538

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (1999) Possible stages in the evolution of the language faculty. Trends Cogn Sci 3:272–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890) The principles of psychology. Henry Holt and Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Just MA, Carpenter PA (1992) A capacity-theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory. Psychol Rev 99(1):122–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerbrat-Orecchioni C (1986) L’Implicite. Armand Colin, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas M, Federmeier KD (2000) Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trens Cogn Sci 4(12):463–470

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis D (1979) Scorekeeping in a language game. J Philos Logic 8:339–359

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus EF (1975) Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychol 7:560–572

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masia V, Canal P, Ricci I, Lombardi Vallauri E and Bambini V (submitted). Presupposition of new information as a pragmatic garden path: evidence from event related brain potentials. J Neurolinguist

  • Osterhout L, Holcomb PH, Swinney DA (1994) Brain potentials elicited by garden path sentences: evidence of the application of verb information during parsing. J Exp Psychol 20(4):786–803

    Google Scholar 

  • Sbisà M (2007) Detto non detto. Le forme della comunicazione implicita, Laterza

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider W, Chein JM (2003) Controlled and automatic processing: behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cogn Sci 27:525–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz F (2014) Presuppositions are fast, whether hard or soft—evidence from the visual world. SALT 24:1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz F (2015) Presupposition versus asserted content in online processing. In: Schwarz F (ed) Experimental perspectives on presupposition. Studies in theoretical psycholinguistics. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 89–108

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz F, Tiemann S (2015) Presupposition projection in online processing. J Semant. http://florianschwarz.net/wp-content/uploads/papers/PresupProjectionProcessing.pdf

  • Shiffrin RM, Schneider W (1977a) Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol Rev 84:1–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin RM, Schneider W (1977b) Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychol Rev 84:127–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin RM, Schneider W (1984) Automatic and controlled processing revisited. Psychol Rev 91(2):269–276

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith K (2008) Is a holistic protolanguage a plausible precursor of language? A test case for a modern evolutionary linguistics. Interact Stud 9(1):1–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (1986/1995) Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell

  • Sperber D, Clément F, Heintz C, Mascaro O, Mercier H, Origgi G, Wilson D (2010) Epistemic vigilance. Mind Lang 25(4):359–393

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stalnaker R (2002) Common ground. Linguist Philos 25:701–721

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strawson PF (1964) Identifying reference and truth-values. Theoria 30(2):96–118. Rep. in Idem, Logico-linguistic papers. London: Methuen 1971: 75–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sturt P, Sandford AJ, Stewart A et al (2004) Linguistic focus and good-enough representations: an application of the change-detection paradigm. Psychol Bull 11:882–888

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J (2003) Evolution of human cognitive architecture. The psychology of learning and motivations, vol 43. Elsevier Science, USA, pp 215–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallerman M (2007) Did our ancestors speak a holistic protolanguage? Lingua 117:279–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson SA (1985) Grammar and written discourse: initial versus final purpose clauses in English. Text 5(1–2):55–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiemann S, Schmid M, Rolke B et al (2011) Psycholinguistic evidence for presuppositions: on-line versus off-line data. In: Reich I, Horch E, Pauly D (eds) Proceedings of sinn and bedeutung 15. Universaar–Saarland University Press, Saarbrücken, pp 581–597

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallauri EL (1995) Tratti linguistici della persuasione in pubblicità. Lingua Nostra 2(3):41–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallauri EL (2009) La struttura informativa. Forma e funzione negli enunciati linguistici, Carocci

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallauri EL (2014) From the knowledge of language to the knowledge of the brain. Ital J Cogn Sci 1:131–161

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallauri EL (2016) The “exaptation” of linguistic implicit strategies. SpringerPlus 5:1106. doi:10.1186/s40064-016-2788-y

  • Vallauri EL, Masia V (2014) Implicitness impact: measuring texts. J Pragmat 61:161–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallauri EL, Masia V (2015) Cognitive constraints on the emergence of topic-focus structure in human communication. In: Chiera A, Ganfi V (eds) Immagine e pensiero. Bilanci nelle scienze cognitive attuali, Corisco, pp 180–204

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang L, Schumacher PB (2013) New is not always costly: evidence from online processing of topic and contrast in Japanese. Front Psychol 4, Article 363, http://www.frontiersin.org/Psychology/editorialboard

  • Wang L, Bastiaansen M, Yang Y, Hagoort P (2011) The influence of information structure on the depth of semantic processing: how focus and pitch accent determine the size of the N400 effect. Neuropsychologia 49(5):813–820

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson D (1975) Presupposition and non-truth-conditional semantics. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Wray A (1998) Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Lang Commun 18:47–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wray A (2002) Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Viviana Masia.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vallauri, E.L., Masia, V. Facilitating Automation in Sentence Processing: The Emergence of Topic and Presupposition in Human Communication. Topoi 37, 343–354 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-016-9417-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-016-9417-9

Keywords

Navigation