Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Roles of Child Gender and Parental Knowledge of Child Development in Parent-Child Interactive Play

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In our study, we aimed to analyse the effect of child gender on parental and child interactive play behaviour, as well as to determine relations between parental general knowledge of child development and parental play behaviour in two developmental periods, namely toddlerhood and early childhood. The sample included 99 children (50 toddlers 1–3 years-old; 49 preschoolers 3–5 years-old) and their parents. Parent–child interactive play with a standard set of toys was observed and assessed in the home setting. We found that parental and child play behaviours were closely related in both age groups. In addition, child’s gender affected child, but not parental, play behaviour such that girls more frequently established the content of play, sustained play frame, and used more symbolic transformations during play than boys did. Parents’ general knowledge of child development was associated with both parental education and parental play behaviour. The findings are applicable to different professionals working with children and their parents in the preschool period.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barbu, S., Cabanes, G., & Le Maner-Idrissi, G. (2011). Boys and girls on the playground: Sex differences in social development are not stable across early childhood. PloS One, 6(1), e16407. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016407.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, J. E., Semenov, A. D., Michaelson, L., Provan, L. S., Snyder, H. R., & Munakata, Y. (2014). Less-structured time in children’s daily lives predicts self-directed executive functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Learning and development of preschool children from the Vygotskian perspective. In A. Kozulin, B. Gindis, V. S. Ageyev, & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Vygotsky’s educational theory in cultural context (pp. 156–176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., & Haynes, O. M. (1998). Vocabulary competence in early childhood: Measurement, latent construct, and predictive validity. Child Development, 69(3), 654–671. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06235.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., O’Reilly, W. A., & Painter, K. M. (1996). Solitary and collaborative pretence play in early childhood: Sources of individual variation in the development of representational competence. Child Development, 67(6), 2910–2929. doi:10.2307/1131759.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., Pascual, L., Painter, K. M., & Galperin, C. (1999). Play in two societies: Pervasiveness of process, specificity of structure. Child Development, 70(2), 317–331. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bredikyte, M. (2011). The zones of proximal development in children’s play. Oulu: University of Oulu, Faculty of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruce, T. (1996). Helping young children to play. Bristol: Hodder Headline Plc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campenni, E. C. (1999). Gender stereotyping of children’s toys: A comparison of parents and nonparents. Sex Roles, 40(1–2), 121–138. doi:10.1023/A:1018886518834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clearfield, M. W., & Nelson, N. M. (2006). Sex differences in mothers’ speech and play behaviour with 6-, 9-, and 14-month-old infants. Sex Roles, 54(1/2), 127–137. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-8874-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cote, L. R., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Child and mother play in three U.S. cultural groups: Comparisons and associations. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(3), 355–363. doi:10.1037/a0015399.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Damast, A. M., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1996). Mother-child play: Sequential interactions and relation between maternal beliefs and behaviours. Child Development, 67(4), 1752–1766. doi:10.2307/1131729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Castro Ribas Jr., R., Seidl de Moura, M. L., & Bornstein, M. H. (2003). Socioeconomic status in Brazilian psychological research: II. Socioeconomic status and parenting knowledge. Estudos de Psicologia, 8(3), 385–392. doi:10.1590/S1413-294X2003000300005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Castro, B. O., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J., & Manhowwer, H. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behaviour: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73(3), 916–934. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeBaryshe, B. D. (1995). Maternal belief systems: Linchpin in the home reading process. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 1–20. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(95)90013-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiPietro, J. A. (1981). Rough and tumble play: A function of gander. Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 50–58. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doyla, G. (2010). Vygotsky in action in the early years. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, J. L., & Pederson, D. R. (1978). Preschool children’s use of objects in symbolic play. Child Development, 49(2), 500–504. doi:10.2307/1128716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pereira, M. P., Wehberg, S., & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326–343. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02042.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. Child Development, 52(4), 1095–1118. doi:10.2307/1129497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., & Reilly, J. S. (1994). MacArthur communicative development inventories. User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, H. (2004). An exploration of children’s play: Classifying play and exploring gender differences in aggressive play (Unpublished dissertation). Coventry: University of Warwick.

  • Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2003). Home literacy environment and phonological awareness in preschool children: Differential effects for rhyme and phoneme awareness. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 24(1), 59–88. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, J. F., Wortham, S., & Reifel, S. (2008). Play and child development (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall/Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, J. H. (1994). Sex differences in toy play and use of video games. In J. H. Goldstein (Ed.), Toys, play and child development (pp. 110–129). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Golombok, S., & Fivush, R. (1994). Gender development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Göncü, A., Patt, M. B., & Kouba, E. (2002). Understanding young children’s pretend play in context. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), Childhood social development (pp. 418–437). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gosso, Y., Salum e Morais, M., & Otta, E. (2007). Pretend play of Brazilian children: A window into different cultural worlds. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(5), 539–558. doi:10.1177/0022022107305237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haight, W. L., & Miller, P. J. (1993). Pretending at home: Early development in sociocultural context. Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hakkarainen, P., & Bredikyte, M. (2008). The zone of proximal development in play and learning. Cultural-Historical Psychology, 4, 2–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1(1), 77–89. doi:10.1080/19312450709336664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P. (2003). We don’t play with toy guns here. London: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzman, L. (2009). Vygotsky at work and play. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, K.-Y., O’Brien Caughy, M., Genevro, J. L., & Miller, T. L. (2005). Maternal knowledge of child development and quality of parenting among White, African-American and Hispanic mothers. Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(2), 149–170. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2004.12.001.

  • Isenberg, J. P., & Jalongo, M. R. (2006). Excerpt from creative thinking and arts-based learning preschool through fourth grade (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, P. (2006). “Rough and tumble” play: Lessons in life. Evolutionary Psychology, 4(1), 330–346. doi:10.1177/147470490600400128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karnik, R. B., & Tudge, J. R. H. (2010). The reality of pretend play: Ethnic, socioeconomic, and gender variations in young children’s involvement. In E. E. Nwokah (Ed.), Play as engagement and communication. Play and culture studies (Vol. 10, pp. 63–81). Lenham, MD: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koustourakis, G. S., Rompola, C., & Asimaki, A. (2015). Rough and tumble play and gender in kindergarten: Perceptions of kindergarten teachers. International Research in Education, 3(2), 93–109. doi:10.5296/ire.v3i2.7570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laakso, M.-L., Poikkeus, A.-M., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, P. (1999). Social interactional behaviours and symbolic play competence as predistors of language development and their associations with maternal attention-directing strategies. Infant Behaviour & Development, 22(4), 541–556. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(00)00022-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, G. W., & Pettit, G. S. (2008). Parenting and the development of children’s peer relationships. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Practical issues in parenting (Vol. 5, pp. 269–304). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, D. E. (2009). Play as learning. In R. Carlisle (Ed.), Encyclopaedia of play in today’s society (pp. 516–521). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J., & Downs, C. (1980). Mothers, fathers, and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviours in young children. Child Development, 51(4), 1237–1247. doi:10.2307/1129566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, V., Boucher, J., Lupton, L., & Watson, S. (2000). Relationships between symbolic play, functional play, verbal and non-verbal ability in young children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(1), 117–127. doi:10.1080/136828200247287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, K. B. (1993). Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacPhee, D. (2002). Manual: Knowledge of infant development inventory. Unpublished manuscript. University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

  • Marjanovič-Umek, L., & Fekonja-Peklaj, U. (2012). Scale for observing child-adult play. Ljubljana: Oddelek za psihologijo, Filozofska fakulteta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marjanovič-Umek, L., Fekonja-Peklaj, U., & Sočan, G. (2012). Home environment as a predictor of child’s language: A mediating role of family literacy activities and symbolic play. Horizons of Psychology, 24, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathieson, K., & Banerjee, R. (2011). Peer play, emotion understanding, and socio-moral explanation: The role of gender. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 188–196. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2010.02020.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCathren, R. B., Warren, S., & Yoder, P. J. (1996). Prelinguistic predictors of later language development. In K. Cole, P. Dale, & D. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (pp. 57–76). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Development, 52(3), 785–797. doi:10.2307/1129078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLoyd, V. C., Warren, D., & Thomas, E. A. C. (1984). Anticipatory and fantastic role enactment in preschool triads. Developmental Psychology, 20(5), 807–814. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.5.807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. A., & Davis, T. L. (1992). Beliefs about children: A comparative study of mothers, teachers, peers, and self. Child Development, 63(5), 1251–1256. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01693.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M., & Christie, T. (2008). Adult modelling facilitates young children’s generation of novel pretend acts. Infant and Child Development, 17(2), 151–162. doi:10.1002/icd.538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nobre-Lima, L., da Luz Vale-Dias, M., Vital Mendes, T., Mónico, L., & MacPhee, D. (2014). The Portuguese version of the knowledge of infant development inventory-P (KIDI-P). European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(6), 740–745. doi:10.1080/17405629.2014.929941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D. (1993). Boys’ rough and tumble play, social competence and group composition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(3), 237–248. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1993.tb00600.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). A longitudinal study of boys’ rough-and-tumble play and dominance during early adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16(1), 77–93. doi:10.1016/0193-3973(95)90017-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., & Galda, L. (1982). The effects of thematic-fantasy play training on the development of children’s story comprehension. American Educational Research Journal, 19(3), 443–452. doi:10.3102/00028312019003443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). The development of play during childhood: Forms and possible functions. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 3(2), 51–57. doi:10.1111/1475-3588.00212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, E., Fei-Yin Ng, F., & Wang, Q. (2004). Gender socialization: A parent x child model. In A. Eagly, A. Beall, & R. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (pp. 120–144). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotsztein, B., & Zelazo, P. R. (2000). The development of sex-typed toy preferences and symbolic play behaviour. Washington, DC: Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185–205. doi:10.1017/S0305000907008343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E., & McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V. (2002). Parent beliefs are cognitions: The dynamic belief systems model. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (Vol. 3, 2nd ed., pp. 485–508). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smidt, S. (2009). Introducing Vygotsky: A guide for practitioners and students in early years education. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smilansky, S., & Shefatya, L. (1990). Facilitating play: A medium for promoting cognitive, socio-emotional and academic development in young children. Gaithersburg, MD: Psychological & Educational Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K. (2007). Pretend play and children’s cognitive and literacy development: Sources of evidence and some lessons from the past. In K. A. Roskos & J. F. Christie (Eds.), Play and literacy in early childhood (pp. 3–36). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1991). Individual variation, correspondence, stability and change in mother and toddler play. Infant Behaviour and Development, 14(2), 143–162. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(91)90002-A.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trawick-Smith, J., & Dziurgot, T. (2011). “Good-fit” teacher-child play interactions and the subsequent autonomous play of preschool children. Early Childhood Quarterly, 26(1), 110–123. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.04.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallentin, M. (2008). Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain & Language., 108(3), 175–183. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.07.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weigel, D. J., Martin, S. S., & Bennet, K. K. (2006). Mother’s literacy beliefs: Connections with the home literacy environment and preschool children’s literacy development. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 6(2), 191–211. doi:10.1177/1468798406066444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, M. (1999). Supporting language and literacy development in the early years. Buckingham: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, E., Desmarais, S., & Gugula, S. (2002). The impact of parenting experience on gender stereotyped toy play of children. Sex Roles, 47(1–2), 39–49. doi:10.1023/A:1020679619728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Urška Fekonja-Peklaj.

Ethics declarations

This research involves human subjects (a sample of children and their parents) and received ethical approval prior to being conducted.

Conflict of Interest

The authors (Ljubica Marjanovič-Umek and Urška Fekonja-Peklaj) declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

All participants gave informed consent to participate in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Marjanovič-Umek, L., Fekonja-Peklaj, U. The Roles of Child Gender and Parental Knowledge of Child Development in Parent-Child Interactive Play. Sex Roles 77, 496–509 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0734-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0734-7

Keywords

Navigation