Skip to main content
Log in

The role of orthographic and phonological processing skills in the reading and spelling of monolingual Persian children

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The main objective of the present study was to examine the contribution of phonological and orthographic skills to Persian reading and spelling. The Persian language is of interest because it has very consistent grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences, but somewhat inconsistent phoneme-to-grapheme correspondences. Reading, spelling, phonological, and orthographic skills were tested in a sample of 109 monolingual Persian students (mean age = 8;1, SD = 4 mo) attending Grade 2 in Iran. The results showed that although monolingual Persian children relied both on phonological and orthographic skills, phonological skills were a strong predictor for both reading and spelling. Another objective of the study was to compare children’s spelling performance in terms of phoneme-to-grapheme (PG) consistencies. As expected, children spelled PG-consistent words more accurately than PG-inconsistent words. Moreover, they relied more on orthographic skills for spelling PG-inconsistent words than for spelling PG-consistent words. The results are discussed in terms of the differential effect of orthographic consistency on reading and spelling.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic-Hebrew-English-speaking Arab children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 611–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arab-Moghaddam, N., & Sénéchal, M. (2001). Orthographic processing skills in reading and spelling in Persian/English bilinguals. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 140–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baluch, B. (1993). Lexical decision in Persian: A test of the orthographic depth hypothesis. International Journal of Psychology, 28, 19–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (1991). Visual word recognition: Evidence for strategic control of lexical and nonlexical routines in oral reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 644–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (2001). Basic process in reading: Semantics affects speeded naming of high-frequency words in an alphabetic script. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 63–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baluch, B., & Shahidi, S. (1991). Visual word recognition in beginning readers of Persian. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1327–1331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (1997). Effects of bilingualism and biliteracy on children’s emerging concept of print. Developmental Psychology, 33, 429–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosman, A. M., & Groot, M. B. (1996). Phonologic mediation is fundamental to reading: Evidence from beginning readers. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 715–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A, & Muller, D. (2005). Phonological recoding and rapid orthographic learning in third-graders’ silent reading: A critical test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 203–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., Genesee, F., & Caravolas, M. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early literacy acquisition. In B. A. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 145–162). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., & Sais, E. (1995). Accelerated metalinguistic (phonological) awareness in bilingual children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 61–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcos, E., & Willows, D. M. (1993). The role of visual processing good in and poor readers’ utilization of orthographic information in letter strings. In S. F. Wright, & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 95–106). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Defior, S., Martos, F., & Cary, L. (2002). Differences in reading acquisition development in two shallow orthographies: Portuguese and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 135–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Defior, S., & Serrano, F. (2005). The initial development of spelling in Spanish: From global to analytical. Reading Writing, 18, 81–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody pictures vocabulary test—revised. Circle Pines, MN:␣America Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1987). Learning to read and spell words. Journal of Reading Behavior, 19, 5–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relation to recoding. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1994). Development of the ability to read words. In R. Ruddell, M. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 323–358). Newark, DE:␣International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, M. Coltheart, & J. Marshall (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., Wimmer, H., & Landerl, K. (1998). Differences in phonological recoding in German- and English-speaking children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 31–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. E. (1990). Phonological skills in learning to read. Hillsdale, NJ:␣Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Porpodas, C., & Wheelwright, S. (1997). Children’s orthographic representations in English and Greek. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 3, 273–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2001). Pseudohomophone effects and phonological recoding procedures in reading development in English and German. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 648–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33, 946–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juul, H. (2005). Knowledge of context sensitive spellings as a component of spelling competence: Evidence from Danish. Applied Psycholinguistics 26, 249–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In R. Frost, & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 67–84). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux. N. K., & Siegel., L. S. (2003). The development of reading in children who speak English as a second language. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1005–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in Kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nassaji, H., & Geva, E. (1999). The contribution of phonological and orthographic processing skills to adult ESL reading: Evidence from native speakers of Farsi. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 241–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Connert. F., Rack, J., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of learning Disabilities, 22, 339–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro, A. M. V. (1995). Reading and spelling development in Brazilian Portuguese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 111–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of experimental child psychology, 29, 294–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processing in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences in to a model of acquisition. Issue in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. L., & Hanley, R. (2003). Effects of orthographic transparency on reading and phoneme awareness in children learning to read in Wales. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2003). Linguistic processes in reading and spelling: The case of alphabetic writing systems: English, French, German and Spanish. In T. Nunes, & P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 43–65). The Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., & Bonnet, P. (1998). Reading and spelling acquisition in French: The role of phonological mediation and orthographic factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 134–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Bechennec, D., & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 194–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steiger, J. H. (1980). Test for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorstad, G. (1991). The effect of orthography in the acquisition of literacy skills. British Journal of Psychology, 82, 527–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Hummer, P. (1990). How German-speaking first graders read and spell: Doubts on the importance of the logographic stage. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 349–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Stone, G. O., & Jacobs, A. M. (1997). What’s the pronunciation for OUGH and the spelling for /u/? A database for computing feed-forward and feedback inconsistency in English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 29, 600–618.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Noriyeh Rahbari.

Appendices

Appendix A

List of words used to test Persian reading and spelling

Appendix B

List of pseudowords used to test phonological processing skills

Appendix C

List of words and pseudohomophones used to test orthographic processing skills

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rahbari, N., Sénéchal, M. & Arab-Moghaddam, N. The role of orthographic and phonological processing skills in the reading and spelling of monolingual Persian children. Read Writ 20, 511–533 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9042-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9042-x

Keywords

Navigation