Skip to main content
Log in

Temporal experience, emotions and decision making in psychopathy

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. For a further phenomenological approach to psychopathy that focusses on issues relating to emotions and self-other-relation see Englebert 2015.

  2. For similar approaches see e.g. Ratcliffe 2015. For a phenomenological approach to psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder see Jacobs 2012, 2014.

  3. Pemmet (2013) refers to this as “acquired sociopathy”, but argues there may also be cases of sociopathy, in which specific moral beliefs of the individual play a central role. I leave this further complication aside here. Jacobs in turn uses the term sociopathy to make clear that her descriptions are based on the ICD and the DSM instead of the Hare scale (Jacobs 2012).

  4. For recent discussions and analysis of these cases see McIlwain 2010; Watson 2013.

  5. As should already become clear in this rough and ready description the rational errors I wish to focus on do not concern the development of sustainable long-term plans, but rather short to medium term decision making. The case of long term decision making has been discussed in detail by Kerrin Jacobs (2012), who draws on Nida-Rümelin’s concept of structural rationality in her discussion.

  6. Of course, the fact that it is part of the PCL-R does not mean that all psychopaths exhibit this tendency. It is in principle possible to be diagnosed as a psychopath without being a pathological liar, insofar as one rates high enough on other items on the checklist.

  7. Thanks are owed to Gwen Adshead for bringing this to my awareness. There may be one problematic exception to this rule. Psychopaths in a prison environment may participate in studies to increase their chances of an early discharge. However, these cases are problematic for two reasons: Subjects here may be motivated to portray themselves as not suffering from any mental condition. They may thus choose to leave researchers in the dark concerning their real motives and experiences. Secondly, psychopaths in a prison environment may (on average) differ from their non-imprisoned peers. For example, their condition may be more severe, or their experience in some other sense atypical, or psychopathy in them might often be mixed with other disorders (such as addiction). All these factors may result in the outcomes of such studies being in some sense skewed.

  8. Although depression is sometimes conceived as a mood disorder under which an individual may suffer for a relatively limited amount of time, this is not the only perspective on the condition, so that similar issues may be of relevance here. See e.g. Jacobs 2013.

  9. The approach I take here is inspired by Owen et al. 2015.

  10. The example is taken from Hare 1999.

  11. This is inspired by Ratcliffe’s talk of a “possibility space” (Ratcliffe 2015). For a discussion of this terminology for the case of psychopathy see Jacobs 2014.

  12. For a differentiation between various forms that this motivational pull can take, see Ratcliffe 2015.

  13. Kennett and Matthews (2009) also mention this idea, without defending it in detail.

  14. Note, however, that Takahashi et al. did not distinguish the reactions of primary and secondary psychopaths, so that it is an open question whether the results are indeed applicable to primary psychopaths. Nevertheless, given the abnormal fear reactions it does seem plausible that this is the case.

  15. There are, however, some limitations here. Lilienfeld et al. 1996 found mixed results on a whole set of temporal tasks administered to psychopaths. Furthermore, we again face the issue that the studies cited did not differentiate between primary and secondary psychopaths.

  16. In this whole passage I rely heavily on the interpretation by Stafford and Gregory (Stafford and Gregory 2006). It should be noted that Heidegger distinguishes difference forms of boredom. I leave these distinctions aside for the purposes of this essay. My aim is not to gain a complete understanding on Heidegger’s account of boredom. Rather my focus is on those parts of the theory that help us to gain a better understanding of psychopathic experience.

  17. I do not want to suggest, of course, that this is the only reason for the aggressive behaviour often seen in psychopaths.

  18. Additionally, there is some indication that boredom may also be relevant for how we value delayed rewards. If we experience time as proceeding much slower than usual, then delays should also seem more costly to us. But if this is the case, then we should find stronger delay discounting in bored individuals (Wittmann and Paulus 2008). While the experiments of Takahashi et al. (2014) indicate that psychopaths do not engage in stronger delay discounting there are some indications that this may not be true for secondary psychopaths (Dean et al. 2013). Further studies differentiating between primary and secondary psychopathy would be needed to substantiate this claim.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC

  • Bauer, L. O. (2001). Antisocial personality disorder and cocaine dependence: their effects on behavioral and electroencephalographic measures of time estimation. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63(1), 87–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorder, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 71, 727–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity. An attempt to clarify some issues about the so-called psychopathic personality. Saint Louis: The C. V. Mosby Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craver, C. F., Kwan, D., Steindam, C., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2014). Individuals with episodic amnesia are not stuck in time. Neuropsychologia, 57, 191–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J. A., & Allman, A.-A. A. (2005). Time flies when you’re having fun: temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain and Cognition, 59(3), 236–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, W. A. (1987). The varieties of fear. Philosophical Studies, 51, 287–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, A. C., Altstein, L. L., Berman, M. E., Constans, J. I., Sugar, C. A., & McCloskey, M. S. (2013). Secondary psychopathy, but not primary psychopathy, is associated with risky decision-making in noninstitutionalized young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(2), 272–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Döring, S. A. (2007). Seeing what to do: affective perception and rational motivation. Dialectica, 61(3), 363–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Englebert, J. (2015). A new understanding of psychopathy: the contribution of phenomenological psychopathology. Psychopathology, 48(6), 368–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. (2004). Emotions and action. In A. S. R. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 158–173). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, T. (2013). Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12, 75–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., Lipnevich, A. A. (2013). Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 401–419.

  • Gordon, R. M. (1980). Fear. The Philosophical Review, 89(4), 560–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience. The disturbing world of the psychopaths among us. New York: The Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., & Thornton, D. (2000). Psychopathy and the predictive validity of the PCL-R: an international perspective. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18, 623–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, R. D., Neumann, C. S., & Widiger, T. A. (2012). Psychopathy. In T. A. Widiger (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of personality disorders (pp. 478–504). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1994). Assessment of psychopathy as a function of age. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(4), 604–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1983). Die Grundbegriffe der Metaphysik. Welt - Endlichkeit - Einsamkeit. Frankfurt am Main: Vittorio Klostermann (Gesamtausgabe, 29/30).

  • Jacobs, K. A. (2012). Soziopathie. Eine Untersuchung moralischer Unfähigkeit. Uelvesbüll: Der Andere Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, K. A. (2013). The depressive situation. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, K. A. (2014). Psychopathic comportment and moral incapacity. In T. Schramme (Ed.), Being amoral. Psychopathy and moral incapacity (pp. 137–166). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kennett, J., & Matthews, S. (2009). Mental time travel, agency, and responsiblity. In M. R. Broome & L. Bortolotti (Eds.), Psychiatry as cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical perspectives (pp. 327–349). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, D., Craver, C. F., Green, L., Myerson, J., Boyer, P., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2012). Future decision-making without episodic mental time travel. Hippocampus, 22(6), 1215–1219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, N. (2014). Psychopaths and blame: the argument from content. Philosophical Psychology, 27(3), 351–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lilienfeld, S. O., Hess, T., & Rowland, C. (1996). Psychopathic personality traits and temporal perspective: a test of the short time horizon hypothesis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 18(3), 285–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maibom, H. L. (2014). Without Fellow Feeling. In T. Schramme (Ed.), Being amoral. Psychopathy and moral incapacity (pp. 91–114). Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McIlwain, D. (2010). Living strangely in time: emotions, masks and morals in psychopathically inclined people. European Journal of Analytic Philosophy, 6(1), 75–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minkowski, E. (1968). Le temps vécu. Études phénoménologiques et psychopathologiques. Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ogloff, J. (2006). Psychopathy/Antisocial Personality Disorder Conundrum, Australian & New Zealand. Journal of Psychiatry 40(6-7), 519–528.

  • Owen, G., Freyenhagen, F., Hotopf, M., & Martin, W. (2015). Temporal inabilities and decision-making capacity in depression. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14, 163–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pemment, J. (2013). Psychopathy versus Sociopathy. Why the Distinction has become Crucial. Aggression and Violent Behavior 18, 458–461.

  • Polaschek, D. L. (2014). Adult criminals with psychopathy: common beliefs about treatability and change have little empirical support. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(4), 296–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poythress, N. G., & Hall, J. R. (2011). Psychopathy and impulsivity reconsidered. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(2), 120–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe, M. (2015). Experiences of depression. A study in phenomenology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skeem, J. L., Poythress, N., Edens, J. F., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Cale, E. M. (2003). Psychopathic personality or personalities? Exploring potential variants of psychopathy and their implications for risk assessment. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 8(5), 513–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stafford, S. P., & Gregory, W. T. (2006). Heidegger’s phenomenology of boredom, and the scientific investigation of conscious experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 5, 155–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi, T., Takagishi, H., Nishinaka, H., Makino, T., & Fukui, H. (2014). Neuroeconomics of psychopathy. Risk taking in probability discounting of gain and loss predicts psychopathy. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 35(6), 510–517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tietjen, R. (2014). Die Wölfe heulen wieder. Selbstreflexive Angst und das Selbst. Talk held at the University of Stuttgart. Stuttgart, 5/19/2014.

  • Vierra, A. (2015). Psychopathy, mental time travel, and legal responsibility. Neuroethics, 1–8.

  • Vogeley, K., & Kupke, C. (2007). Disturbances of time consciousness from a phenomenological and a neuroscientific perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(1), 157–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, G. (2013). Psychopathic agency and prudential deficits. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society CXIII, 269–292.

  • Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(4), 669–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widom, C. S., & Newman, J. P. (1985). Characteristics of non-institutionalized psychopaths. In P. D. Farrington & J. Gunn (Eds.), Aggression and dangerousness (pp. 57–80). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M. (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences, 364(1525), 1955–1967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 7–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yildrim, B. O., & Derksen, J. J. (2015). Clarifying the heterogeneity in psychopathic samples: towards a new continuum of primary and secondary psychopathy. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 24, 9–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zagzebski, L. (2003). Emotion and moral judgment. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 66(1), 104–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, D. (2014). Psychological time as information: the case of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anja Berninger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Berninger, A. Temporal experience, emotions and decision making in psychopathy. Phenom Cogn Sci 16, 661–677 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-016-9466-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-016-9466-y

Keywords

Navigation