Skip to main content
Log in

The role of tense and agreement in the licensing of subjects: evidence from participial clauses in Bangla

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines the subject-licensing properties of Bangla non-finite participles. In this paper, I show that tense plays a pivotal role in explaining the distribution of subjects in participles. Three participles, the imperfective participle, the perfective participle and the conditional participle, have been examined regarding their different choices of (c)overt subjects, and their compatibility with different types of control. Following the ‘T-Agr calculus’ of Landau (2000, 2004), I show that tense unifies the different types of participles. Clausal selection along with the presence of tense, also has a crucial role in the distribution of subjects across the participles. I suggest that tense is not only vital in licensing the subjects, but also pivotal for their compatibility with different types of predicates. Additionally, I show that a temporal restriction, in the guise of covert agreement, motivates different subject selection in the participles in Bangla.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Bangla (Bengali) is spoken primarily in the Indian states of West Bengal and Tripura, and in Bangladesh. The data in this paper represents a variety spoken near Kolkata, which is considered to be the standard variety spoken in West Bengal.

  2. Gerundives in Bangla, morphologically marked with -(w)a-(no), appear in the subject position whereas none of the participial clauses do, as illustrated below.

    1. (i)
      figure b
  3. Here and elsewhere ec marks the empty category that is the subject of the participial clause. The lexical subjects in the matrix clause are in morphologically null nominative case, which I do not specify in the glosses.

  4. Overt subjects in non-volitional PPL clauses (van Der Wurff 1989; Bagchi 1993, 2005) are illustrated below:

    1. (i)
      figure d

    These non-volitional predicates are unaccusative in nature. Although the events in the participle and the matrix clause are sequential, like in their null subject counterparts, the events share an implicit causal relationship. (See Bagchi 2005 for details.) The licensing of overt unaccusative subjects and the connection with volitionality remains to be explained.

  5. Backward control is reported for similar examples of conjunctive participles in Assamese (a closely related language). The controller is argued to be in the embedded clause while the empty subject is the matrix subject (Haddad 2007). Case-mismatch between the subjects of the matrix and embedded clause is a diagnostic of backward control. However, such case-mismatch, common in Assamese, is not possible in the current variety of Bangla. Thus, the possibility of backward control can be ruled out in Bangla.

  6. A reduplicated imperfective participle gives rise to different readings. For example, when a telic verb is reduplicated, the matrix event instantly follows the participial event. A simultaneous temporal reading arises when an imperfective participle of an atelic verb is reduplicated. For example, the event of ‘ringing bell’ is simultaneous with ‘seeing the man’, as in (i). The reduplication does not occur with a purpose reading.

    1. (i)
      figure i
  7. This diagnostic fails in conditional correlatives (Bhatt 2003) and apparently in subjunctive clauses (Bagchi 1991; Dasgupta 1996, 2005 and Bhattacharya 2013). However, Probal Dasgupta (p.c.) states that the diagnostic does not hold because the subjunctives are not head-final structures.

  8. The disjoint reference of the null subject from the matrix subject in the given word order is not natural. However, if the embedded clause is extraposed, such a reading is available when appropriate contextual information is provided. See Simpson et al. (2012) for a similar observation. Positioning of embedded clauses with respect to the verb (i.e., pre-verbal or post-verbal) often influences the interpretation in Bangla.

    Context given: Rick has been persistent that Sudha takes medicine, although Sudha doesn’t want to do so.

    1. (i)
      figure r
  9. The PROarb, which is not controlled, appears only with gerunds or verbal nouns in Bangla.

  10. However, as an anonymous reviewer suggests, the temp-IPL and the CPL might also allow an OC PRO, in addition to allowing a pro. In contexts where the null subject is co-referential with the matrix subject, there is no certain way to distinguish between a pro and a PRO as the pro has a superset of properties of the obligatory controlled PRO. The availability of the pro subject can be proved, but the unavailability of the OC PRO subject cannot be disproved. Thus, if a pro-subject is available in the temp-IPL and CPL, an OC PRO-subject might be available as well.

  11. Pro-drop, non-argument ellipsis languages (e.g., Spanish, Italian) allow strict readings and sloppy readings only of a certain kind. This is illustrated for Spanish below, reproducing examples from Simpson et al. Example (ib) allows a strict reading, with the null subject of the embedded clause interpreted as “Maria’s daughter”. It crucially does not allow a sloppy reading with the null subject interpreted as “Juan’s daughter”. This is typical for a pro subject. (pro, of course, also allows co-reference with Juan, which can be thought of as a sloppy reading of a different kind, not relevant for Simpson et al’s argument, but is expected, given everything said in this section.)

    1. (i)
      figure ac
  12. Overt DPs in the ‘want’-class predicates can occur in a benefactive construction (i), which is not a control structure, and the same construction is possible for try-predicates too.

    1. (i)
      figure aj

References

  • Anand, Pranav. 2006. De de se. PhD dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.

  • Bagchi, Tista. 1991. Conditionals and emphasizers in Bangla: Some pragmatic effects of their interaction. Paper at the Thirteenth South Asian languages analysis roundtable. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Illinois.

  • Bagchi, Tista. 1993. Clausal subordination in Bangla: A cross-modular approach. PhD dissertation, University of Chicago, Illinois.

  • Bagchi, Tista. 2005. Causation and tense in subordinate clauses: Conjunctive participles in Bangla and Hindi. In Polymorphous linguistics: Jim McCawley’s legacy, eds. Salikoko M. Mufwene, Elaine J. Francis, and Rebecca S. Wheeler, 109–134. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhatt, Rajesh. 2003. Topics in the syntax of the modern Indo-Aryan languages: Negation and negative polarity. Handout downloaded from: web.mit.edu/rbhatt/www/24.956/neg.pdf.

  • Bhattacharya, Tanmoy. 2013. The subjunctive in Bangla. In The syntax and semantics of mood and tense, eds. Alexandra Giorgi, James Higginbotham, and Fabio Pianesi. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Draft downloaded from: http://people.du.ac.in/~tanmoy/bergamo.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, Denis. 1984. On the content of empty categories. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, Joan. 1972. Theory of complementation in English syntax. PhD dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam, and Howard Lasnik. 1993. The theory of principles and parameters. In Syntax: An international handbook of contemporary research, eds. Joachim Jacobs, Arnim von Stechow, Wolfgang Sternefeld, and Theo Vennemann, 506–569. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dan, Mina. 1989. Gerunds, action nominals, and a few degrees of nominalization in Bangla. Indian Journal of Linguistics 16: 21–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, Probal. 1996. Remarks on subjunctivity: Perspectives on language in society. In Papers on language and society (Papers in memory of Prof. R. N. Srivastav), eds. Shivendra K. Verma and Dilip Singh, 72–89. Delhi: Kalinga.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, Probal. 2005. Q-baa and Bangla clause structure. In Yearbook of South Asian languages and linguistics 2005, eds. Singh Rajendra, et al., 45–81. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • del Prado Núñez, Z., and James Gair. 1994. The position of negation in Bengali: An account of synchronic and diachronic variation. Cornell Working Papers in Linguistics 12: 127–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, Donca. 1992. On obviation. In CSLI lecture notes: Lexical matters, eds. Ivan. A. Sag and Anna Szabolcsi, 85–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghosh, Sanjukta. 2002. The syntax-pragmatics interface of Bangla. PhD dissertation, University of Hyderabad, Hyderabad.

  • Haddad, Youssef A. 2007. Adjunct control in Telugu and Assamese. PhD dissertation. University of Florida, Florida.

  • Jaeggli, Osvaldo, and Kenneth J. Safir. 1989. The null subject parameter and parametric theory. In The null subject parameter, eds. Jaeggli Osvaldo and Ken Safir, 1–44. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Klaiman, Miriam H. 1983. Bengali conjunctive participle constructions. In Proceedings of the ninth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 138–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klaiman, Miriam H. 1981. Volitionality and subject in Bengali: A study on semantics parameters in grammatical processes. PhD dissertation, University of Chicago, Illinois. Distributed by the Indiana University linguistics Club.

  • Landau, Idan. 2000. Elements of control: Structure and meaning in infinitival constructions. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, Idan. 2004. The scale of finiteness and the calculus of control. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 23: 811–877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, Idan. 2006. Severing the distribution of PRO from case. Syntax 9: 153–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, David. 1979. Attitudes de dicto and de se. Philosophical Review 88(4): 513–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manzini, Rita. 1983. On control and control theory. Linguistic Inquiry 14: 421–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, Alec. 2000. Case and licensing. In Arguments and case: Explaining Burzio’s generalization, ed. Eric Reuland, 1–30. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, Roger A. 2001. Null case and the distribution of PRO. Linguistic Inquiry 32: 141–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, Roger A. 1996. A minimalist theory of PRO and control. PhD dissertation, University of Connecticut, Connecticut.

  • Masica, Colin P. 1976. Defining a linguistic area: South Asia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, Thomas. 2004. The position of morphological case in the derivation: A study on the syntax-morphology interface. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.

  • Pesetsky, David. 1991. Zero syntax. Vol. 2 of Infinitives. Unpublished manuscript. Downloaded from: http://web.mit.edu/linguistics/www/pesetsky/infins.pdf.

  • Ramchand, Gillian C. 2003. Two types of negation in Bengali. In Clause structure in South Asian languages, eds. Veneeta Dayal and Anoop Mahajan, 39–67. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, Gillian C. 2013. Deriving variable linearization. A commentary on Simpson and Syed (2013). Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. doi:10.1007/s11049-013-9225-5.

  • Reinhart, Tanya, and Eric Reuland. 1993. Reflexivity. Linguistic Inquiry 24: 657–720.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuland, Eric, and Tanya Reinhart. 1995. Pronouns, anaphors and case. In Studies in comparative Germanic syntax, eds. Hubert Haider, Susan Olsen, and Sten Vikner, 241–268. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurðsson, Halldór Á. 2008. The case of PRO. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 26: 403–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurðsson, Halldór Á. 2012. Minimalist C/case. Linguistic Inquiry 43: 191–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigurðsson, Halldór Á. 1989. Verbal syntax and the case in Icelandic. PhD dissertation, Lund University.

  • Simpson, Andrew, and Saurov Syed. 2013. Finiteness, negation and the directionality of headedness in Bangla. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory. doi:10.1007/s11049-013-9223-7.

  • Simpson, Andrew, Arunima Choudhury, and Mythili Menon. 2012. South Asian perspectives on argument ellipsis. In Proceedings of the 14th Seoul international conference on generative grammar, 370–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stowell, Tim. 1983. The tense of infinitives. Linguistic Inquiry 13(3): 561–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundaresan, Sandhya, and Thomas McFadden. 2009. Subject distribution in Tamil and other languages: Selection vs. case. Journal of South Asian Linguistics 2(1): 5–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tonhauser, Judith. 2006. The temporal semantics of noun phrases: Evidence from Guaraní. PhD Dissertation, Stanford University, California.

  • Trivedi, Ramendrasundar. 1905/1950. Naa [Not]. In Raamenda Racanaabalii, Vol. 3, 91–96. Kolkata: Bangiya Sahitya Parisat.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Wurff, Wim. 1989. The syntax of participial adjuncts in Eastern Bengali. Journal of Linguistics 25: 373–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varlokosta, Spyridoula, and Norbert Hornstein. 1993. Control in Modern Greek. In Proceedings of the 23rd meeting of the North Eastern Linguistic Society, ed. Amy J. Schafer, 507–521. Amherst: GLSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ylikoski, Jussi. 2003. Defining non-finites: Action nominals, converbs and infinitives. SKY Journal of Linguistics 16: 185–237.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am indebted to Roumyana Pancheva for her guidance throughout the project. Without her invaluable comments, suggestions and corrections, this paper would not exist. Special thanks to Thomas McFadden for many helpful suggestions on the first draft, and to the two anonymous reviewers, whose comments improved the paper. Thanks also to Tista Bagchi, Probal Dasgupta, and Andrew Simpson for reading and commenting on the first draft. Detailed editorial comments on content and formatting by Sandhya Sundaresan helped immensely towards improving the paper. Thanks to Arunima Choudhury for checking the data and Christina Hagedorn for proofreading the preliminary draft. Finally, comments and suggestions from the audience of FiSAL at the University of Tromsø were very helpful as well. Of course, all errors are mine.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Priyanka Biswas.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Biswas, P. The role of tense and agreement in the licensing of subjects: evidence from participial clauses in Bangla. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 32, 87–113 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-013-9212-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-013-9212-x

Keywords

Navigation