Skip to main content
Log in

The "Efficiency" Effect of Conceptual Referents on the Generation of Happiness: A Cross-National Analysis

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper analyses how different definitions of happiness affect the individual assessment of subjective wellbeing. Bearing in mind the production theory, we study the impact of definitions of happiness on how efficiently a set of determinants are used and converted into happiness. From a theoretical point of view, we discuss the role of the Conceptual Referent Theory within the affective-cognitive process of personal evaluation of happiness. We use data from the Understanding High Happiness in Latin America survey (2018), which includes a sample of people from Colombia, Costa Rica, Mexico and the USA, to investigate how happiness conceptual referents influence personal assessment using Stochastic Frontier Analysis. This method permits to assess the impact of different definitions of happiness on how a set of standard drivers like income and social relations are used and converted into perceived happiness. To this end, conceptual referents for happiness are treated as (in)efficiency factors. This study shows that the concept of happiness shapes how individuals value the same inputs. “Stoicism”, “virtue” and “enjoyment” emerge as the most efficient referents. Also, we observe that cross-country differences in the level of perceived happiness are associated with differences in the prevalence of conceptual referents. In the USA, where less efficient definitions prevail, people report lower levels of happiness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Source: Own elaboration based on Veenhoven (2009)

Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This approach has an interesting parallel with Fredrickson's Broaden and Build Theory (2004). Happiness is not the result of pure calculus, but it is inferred from the continuity of daily experiences, that is, it is a heuristic of personal experiences (Schwartz and Strack 1991). While positive experiences broaden personal capabilities and exploratory behaviour, negative emotions are related to attitudes of survival. Happy people are those who feel good most of the time. Thus, the frequency of positive affective states, rather than the intensity, is the critical determinant (Diener et al. 1991). However, positive emotions are transient and subdue individuals to the hedonic treadmill. The achievement of life satisfaction depends on a combination of positive emotions and the generation of stable resources. In this sense, what Frederickson et al. (2008) links with meditation but in a more ample view could be assimilated with any form on reflection on own experiences plays a prominent role, that is, the ability to build a complete sense of life itself.

  2. We have checked the association between relative income and educational levels. There is a statistically significant difference between levels, as determined by one-way ANOVA [F(4, 3806) = 39.08, p = 0.000]. A Tukey post-hoc test revealed that relative income was statistically significantly higher in the group with university education compared to every other group ( p < 0.000 in any comparison between groups). However, there were no statistically significant differences between the first four groups. These results suggest that the high correlation is mainly driven by people with improved education, who, in our sample, constitute a higher proportion of US residents compared to Latin America. The uneven cross-country distribution of university education, which mirrors the distribution of happiness, may explain, at least in part, the results of Model 1. Given the aim of this paper, we have opted to avoid this issue by excluding education from our preferred specification of the production function.

  3. It should be noted, however, that our results are robust for the estimation of production functions and technical efficiency by countries, or group of countries. By applying the same methodology, the estimation of technical efficiency for Latin American countries and the USA separately shows that the relative efficiency of conceptual referents remains unchanged. That is, both for Latin America and the USA, it is possible to identify the first group of most efficient definitions of happiness (stoicism, enjoyment and virtue), utopism at the lower end, and the rest of referents fall in the middle of this ranking. Therefore, though this paper studies efficiency at the individual level, and it does not allow for analysing heterogeneity, these estimations show that the efficiency of conceptual referents appears to be stable across countries. Results of this robustness check are available upon request.

References

  • Aigner, D., Lovell, C. K., & Schmidt, P. (1977). Formulation and estimation of stochastic frontier production function models. Journal of Econometrics, 6(1), 21–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M. (2001). The psychology of happiness. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Battese, G. E., & Coelli, T. J. (1988). Prediction of firm-level technical efficiencies with a generalized frontier production function and panel data. Journal of Econometrics, 38(3), 387–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P. W. (1990). Recent developments in the econometric estimation of frontiers. Journal of Econometrics, 46(1–2), 39–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binder, M., & Broekel, T. (2012). Happiness no matter the cost? An examination on how efficiently individuals reach their happiness levels. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(4), 621–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchflower, D. G., & Oswald, A. J. (2004). Money, sex and happiness: An empirical study. Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 106(3), 393–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, T. N., & Lang, K. (2019). The sad truth about happiness scales. Journal of Political Economy, 127(4), 1629–1640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Etilé, F., Postel-Vinay, F., Senik, C., & Van der Straeten, K. (2005). Heterogeneity in reported well-being: Evidence from twelve European countries. Economic Journal, 115(502), C118–C132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., & Lelkes, O. (2005). Deliver us from evil: Religion as insurance. HAL.

  • Coelli, T., & Perelman, S. (1999). A comparison of parametric and non-parametric distance functions: With application to European railways. European Journal of Operational Research, 117(2), 326–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cordero, J. M., Salinas-Jiménez, J., & Salinas-Jiménez, M. M. (2017). Exploring factors affecting the level of happiness across countries: A conditional robust nonparametric frontier analysis. European Journal of Operational Research, 256(2), 663–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cordero, J. M., Polo, C., & Salinas-Jiménez, J. (2020). Subjective well-being and heterogeneous contexts: A cross-national study using semi-nonparametric frontier methods. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1–20.

  • Cullinane, K., & Song, D. W. (2006). Estimating the relative efficiency of European container ports: A stochastic frontier analysis. Research in Transportation Economics, 16, 85–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Debnath, R. M., & Shankar, R. (2014). Does good governance enhance happiness: a cross nation study. Social Indicators Research, 116(1), 235–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Del Corral, J. (2012). Eficiencia en el deporte: Entrenadores en la Primera División del fútbol español 2009–2011. Estudios de Economía Aplicada, 30(2), 545–563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Tella, R., MacCulloch, R. J., & Oswald, A. J. (2001). Preferences over inflation and unemployment: Evidence from surveys of happiness. American Economic Review, 91(1), 335–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, M. A., & Sánchez, R. (2008). Firm size and productivity in Spain: A stochastic frontier analysis. Small Business Economics, 30(3), 315–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings, and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 926–935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Pavot, W., & Sandvik, E. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not intensity of positive and negative affect. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Subjective wellbeing: An interdisciplinary perspective. London: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(1), 94–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dong, Y., Hamilton, R., & Tippett, M. (2014). Cost efficiency of the Chinese banking sector: A comparison of stochastic frontier analysis and data envelopment analysis. Economic Modelling, 36, 298–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In P. A. David & W. R. Melvin (Eds.), Nations and households in economic growth (pp. 98–125). Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, M. J. (1957). The measurement of productive efficiency. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (General), 120(3), 253–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., & Frijters, P. (2004). How important is methodology for the estimates of the determinants of happiness? The Economic Journal, 114(497), 641–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A., & Gowdy, J. M. (2007). Environmental degradation and happiness. Ecological Economics, 60(3), 509–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1045–1062.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2002). What can economists learn from happiness research? Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 402–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Stutzer, A. (2005). Happiness research: State and prospects. Review of Social Economy, 63(2), 207–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C., & Chattopadhyay, S. (2013). Gender and well-being around the world. International Journal of Happiness and Development, 1(2), 212–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, H. (2009). Understanding health inequalities. McGraw-Hill Education.

  • Greene, W. (1997). Frontier Production Functions. In M. H. Pesaran & P. Schmidt (Eds.), Handbook of applied econometrics. Volume II: Microeconomics. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gutiérrez, E., Lozano, S., & Furió, S. (2014). Evaluating efficiency of international container shipping lines: A bootstrap DEA approach. Maritime Economics and Logistics, 16(1), 55–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F. (2003). How’s life? Combining individual and national variables to explain subjective well-being. Economic modelling, 20(2), 331–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F., & Barrington-Leigh, C. P. (2010). How much is social capital worth? Cambridge: National Bureau of Economic Research.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of well–being. Philosophical transactions of the royal society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1435–1446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingsworth, B. (2003). Non-parametric and parametric applications measuring efficiency in health care. Health Care Management Science, 6(4), 203–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. New York, NY: Science.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, R. (2001). Alternative methods to examine hospital efficiency: Data envelopment analysis and stochastic frontier analysis. Health Care Management Science, 4(2), 103–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jebb, A. T., Tay, L., Diener, E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Happiness, income satiation and turning points around the world. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 33–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kesebir, P., & Diener, E. (2008). Pursuit of happiness: Empirical answers to philosophical questions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(2), 117–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. New York: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lelkes, O. (2006). Knowing what is good for you: Empirical analysis of personal preferences and the “objective good.” The Journal of Socio-Economics, 35(2), 285–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louis, V. V., & Zhao, S. (2002). Effects of family structure, family SES, and adulthood experiences on life satisfaction. Journal of Family Issues, 23(8), 986–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, R. E., & Gohm, C. L. (2000). Age and sex differences in subjective well-being across cultures. In E. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Culture and subjective well-being (pp. 291–317). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S. (2001). Why are some people happier than others? The role of cognitive and motivational processes in well-being. American Psychologist, 56, 239–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martín, F., Palomino, I., & Vivanco, M. G. (2012). Combination of measured and modelling data in air quality assessment in Spain. International Journal of Environment and Pollution, 49(1–2), 36–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meeusen, W., & van den Broeck, J. (1977). Technical efficiency and dimension of the firm: Some results on the use of frontier production functions. Empirical Economics, 2(2), 109–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nikolova, M., & Popova, O. (2020). Sometimes your best just ain’t good enough: The worldwide evidence on subjective well-being efficiency. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy. https://doi.org/10.1515/bejeap-2019-0396.

  • Oishi, S., Graham, J., Kesebir, S., & Galinha, I. (2013). Concepts of happiness across time and culture. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(5), 559–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, A. J. (1997). Happiness and economic performance. The Economic Journal, 107(445), 1815–1831.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pichler, F. (2006). Subjective quality of life of young Europeans. Feeling happy but who knows why? Social Indicators Research75(3), 419–444.

  • Rodríguez, L. (2001). Bienestar e ingreso: Un estudio sobre el concepto de felicidad. Puebla: Universidad de las Américas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rojas, M. (2005). A conceptual-referent theory of happiness: Heterogeneity and its consequences. Social Indicators Research, 74(2), 261–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rojas, M. (2007). Heterogeneity in the relationship between income and happiness: A conceptual referent theory explanation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosko, M. D., & Mutter, R. L. (2008). Stochastic frontier analysis of hospital inefficiency: A review of empirical issues and an assessment of robustness. Medical Care Research and Review, 65(2), 131–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1998). Successful aging. New York, Pantheon: Dell Pub.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schröder, C., & Yitzhaki, S. (2017). Revisiting the evidence for cardinal treatment of ordinal variables. European Economic Review, 92, 337–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Strack, N. (1991). Evaluating one’s life: A judgment model of subjective well-being. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective wellbeing (pp. 27–48). Pergamon, London: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sen, A. (1993). Capability and well-being. In M. C. Nussbaum & A. Sen (Eds.), The quality of life (pp. 30–53). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Simar, L., & Wilson, P. W. (1998). Sensitivity analysis of efficiency scores: How to bootstrap in nonparametric frontier models. Management science, 44(1), 49–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simar, L., & Wilson, P. W. (2000). Statistical inference in nonparametric frontier models: The state of the art. Journal of Productivity Analysis, 13(1), 49–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sortheix, F., & Schwartz, S. (2017). Values that underlie and undermine well-being: Variability across countries. European Journal of Personality, 31, 187–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. E. (1980). Likelihood functions for generalized stochastic frontier estimation. Journal of Econometrics, 13(1), 57–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stutzer, A. (2004). The role of income aspirations in individual happiness. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 54(1), 89–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titus, M. A., & Eagan, K. (2016). Examining production efficiency in higher education: The utility of stochastic frontier analysis. In L. W. Perna & M. B. Paulsen (Eds.), Higher education: Handbook of theory and research (pp. 441–512). Cham: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Understanding High Happiness in Latin America. (2018). Retrived from https://www.happinessandwellbeing.org/rojas.

  • Veenhoven, R. (1984). Conditions of happiness. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (1997). Advances in understanding happiness. Revue Québécoise de Psychologie, 18(2), 29–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (2001). What we know about happiness. Rotterdam: Erasmus University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (2009). How do we assess how happy we are? Tenets, implications and tenability of three theories. In A. Dutt & B. Ratcliff (Eds.), Happiness, economics and politics. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenhoven, R. (2010). How universal is happiness? In E. Diener, J. F. Helliwell, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 328–350). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge Mariano Rojas for the opportunity to analyse data from the "Understanding High Happiness in Latin America: Human Relations and Spirituality in a Life Well Lived", a project funded by the John Templeton Foundation and Saint Louis University. We thank the editor and the reviewers for their careful reading of our manuscript and their many insightful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Atilano Pena-López.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human or Animal Rights

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed directly by any of the authors. It makes use of general population survey data from the study “Understanding High Happiness in Latin America: Human Relations and Spirituality in a Life Well Lived,” granted as part of the “Happiness and Well-Being: Integrating Research Across the Disciplines” project, based at Saint Louis University and financed by the John Templeton Foundation.

Ethical Approval

The compliance with ethical standards is presumed by the rules of participation in the above-mentioned project, and guaranteed by the project PI.

Funding

This project has received the financial support of the Ministerio de Economía, Industria y Competitividad, Spain (Project CSO2017-86178-R).

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix

Social relations: Principal Component Analysis

The Understanding Happiness in Latin America survey (2018) includes a large battery of questions about social relations and social life. Since social relations are a major driver of happiness (see, for example, Helliwell 2003; Pichler 2006; Lelker 2006), we have used these questions to obtain four proxies of individual social networks. In particular, Principal Component Analysis (PCA) has been applied as a dimensionality reduction technique to 14 different questions, which are presented in Table 7. These questions are aimed at obtaining information about the frequency of interactions with different social ties as well as participation in civic and religious organizations. Using oblique rotation, and by applying the standard “eigenvalue > criteria” (see Fig. 6), four factors have been retained. These factors are coherent with theoretical considerations. They include questions respectively related to work relations (variable RELATIONS-WORK), social interactions with friends and neighbours (RELATIONS-FRIENDS), family ties (RELATION-FAMILY), and to participation in associations (RELATIONS-ASSOCIATIONS). These factors account for about 57% of the total variance. Table 7 presents the pattern matrix. Social relations variables included as inputs in the production function are the estimated normalised individual factor scores.

Table 7 Principal component analysis: pattern matrix
Fig. 6
figure 6

Principal component analysis: number of components and eigenvalues

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pena-López, A., Rungo, P. & López-Bermúdez, B. The "Efficiency" Effect of Conceptual Referents on the Generation of Happiness: A Cross-National Analysis. J Happiness Stud 22, 2457–2483 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00328-3

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-020-00328-3

Keywords

Navigation