Skip to main content
Log in

A Mathematical Model of Prediction-Driven Instability: How Social Structure Can Drive Language Change

  • Published:
Journal of Logic, Language and Information Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

I discuss a stochastic model of language learning and change. During a syntactic change, each speaker makes use of constructions from two different idealized grammars at variable rates. The model incorporates regularization in that speakers have a slight preference for using the dominant idealized grammar. It also includes incrementation: The population is divided into two interacting generations. Children can detect correlations between age and speech. They then predict where the population’s language is moving and speak according to that prediction, which represents a social force encouraging children not to sound out-dated. Both regularization and incrementation turn out to be necessary for spontaneous language change to occur on a reasonable time scale and run to completion monotonically. Chance correlation between age and speech may be amplified by these social forces, eventually leading to a syntactic change through prediction-driven instability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adger D. (2003) Core syntax: A minimalist approach. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, J., & Nikitina, T. (2007). The gradience of the dative alternation. In L. Uyechi, & L. H. Wee (Eds.), Reality exploration and discovery: Pattern interaction in language and life. Stanford: CSLI Publiscations. http://www.stanford.edu/~bresnan/publications/index.html.

  • Briscoe E. J. (2000) Grammatical acquisition: Inductive bias and coevolution of language and the language acquisition device. Language 76(2): 245–296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briscoe, E. J. (2002). Grammatical acquisition and linguistic selection. In E. J. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic evolution through language acquisition: Formal and computational models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. http://www.cl.cam.ac.uk/users/ejb/creo-evol.ps.gz.

  • Ellegård A. (1953) The auxiliary do: The establishment and regulation of its use in English, Gothenburg studies in English. Almqvist and Wiksell, Stockholm

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson E., Wexler K. (1994) Triggers. Linguistic Inquiry 25: 407–454

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold E. M. (1967) Language identification in the limit. Information and Control 10: 447–474

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson Kam C. L., Newport E. L. (2005) Regularizing unpredictable variation: The roles of adult and child learners in language formation and change. Language Learning and Development 1(2): 151–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby S. (2001) Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure: An iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation 5(2): 102–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Komarova N. L., Niyogi P., Nowak M. A. (2001) The evolutionary dynamics of grammar acquisition. Journal of Theoretical Biology 209(1): 43–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroch A. (1989) Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change. Language Variation and Change 1: 199–244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov W. (1994) Principles of linguistic change: Internal factors. Blackwell, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov W. (2001) Principles of linguistic change: Social factors. Blackwell, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchener W. G. (2003) Bifurcation analysis of the fully symmetric language dynamical equation. Journal of Mathematical Biology 46: 265–285. doi:10.1007/s00285-002-0172-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchener W. G. (2007) Game dynamics with learning and evolution of universal grammar. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 69(3): 1093–1118. doi:10.1007/s11538-006-9165-x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchener, W. G. (2009a). Mean-field and measure-valued differential equation models for language variation and change in a spatially distributed population, submitted.

  • Mitchener, W. G. (2009b). A stochastic model of language change through social structure and prediction-driven instability, submitted.

  • Mitchener W. G., Nowak M. A. (2003) Competitive exclusion and coexistence of universal grammars. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 65(1): 67–93. doi:10.1006/bulm.2002.0322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchener W. G., Nowak M. A. (2004) Chaos and language. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences 271(1540): 701–704. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niyogi P. (1998) The informational complexity of learning. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Niyogi P., Berwick R. C. (1996) A language learning model for finite parameter spaces. Cognition 61: 161–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niyogi, P., & Berwick, R. C. (1997). A dynamical systems model for language change. Complex Systems, 11, 161–204, ftp://publications.ai.mit.edu/ai-publications/1500-1999/AIM-1515.ps.Z.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak M. A., Komarova N. L., Niyogi P. (2001) Evolution of universal grammar. Science 291(5501): 114–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowak M. A., Komarova N. L., Niyogi P. (2002) Computational and evolutionary aspects of language. Nature 417(6889): 611–617

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford A. (2004) Minimalist syntax: Exploring the structure of English. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tesar B., Smolensky P. (2000) Learnability in optimality theory. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner A. (2005) Why DO dove: Evidence for register variation in early modern English negatives. Language Variation and Change 17: 257–280. doi:10.1017/S0954394505050106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang C. D. (2002) Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to W. Garrett Mitchener.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mitchener, W.G. A Mathematical Model of Prediction-Driven Instability: How Social Structure Can Drive Language Change. J of Log Lang and Inf 20, 385–396 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-011-9136-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-011-9136-y

Keywords

Navigation