Skip to main content
Log in

The Importance of Temperament for Understanding Early Manifestations of Autism Spectrum Disorder in High-Risk Infants

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated the relationship between infant temperament characteristics and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) risk status. Temperament was examined at 6, 12, and 24 months in 282 infants at high familial risk for ASD and 114 low-risk controls using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire-Revised and Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Infants were divided into three groups at 24 months: High-Risk Positive—classified as ASD (HR Pos), High-Risk Negative (HR Neg), and Low-Risk Negative (LR Neg). At 6 and 12 months HR Pos infants exhibited lower Surgency and Regulatory Capacity than LR Neg infants. By 12 months they also demonstrated increased Negative Affect. Group differences remained, when early signs of ASD were controlled for, suggesting that temperament differences could be useful targets for understanding the development of ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bieberich, A. A., & Morgan, S. B. (1998). Brief report: Affective expression in children with autism or down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(4), 333–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 237–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, P. F., Golding, J., Edmond, A., & Steer, C. D. (2012). Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: Precursors and early signs. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(3), 249–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., Garon, N., Roberts, W., Smith, I. M., Szatmari, P., et al. (2008). Clinical assessment of autism in high-risk 18-month-olds. Autism, 12(5), 433–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., McDermott, C., Rombough, V., & Brian, J. (2008). The autism observation scale for infants: Scale development and reliability data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 731–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1995). The Carey temperament scales. Scottsdale, AZ: Behavioral-Developmental Initiatives.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, S. M., Hudry, D., Elsabbagh, M., Charman, T., Johnson, M. H., & Team, B. A. S. I. S. (2013). Temperament in the first 2 years of life in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 673–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • del Rosario, M., Gillespie-Lynch, K., Johnson, S., Sigman, M., & Hutman, T. (2013). Parent-reported temperament trajectories among infant siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 381–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeYoung, C. G. (2006). Higher-order factors of the Big Five in a multi-informant sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 1138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elison, J. T., Paterson, S. J., Wolff, J., Reznick, S., Sasson, N J Gu, Botteron, H., et al. (2013). White matter microstructure and atypical visual orienting in 7 month-olds at risk for autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 899–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elison, J., Wolff, J. J., Reznick, J. S., Botteron, K. N., Estes, A. M., Gu, H., et al. (2014). Repetitive behavior in 12-month-olds later classified with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(11), 1216–1224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., Gu, H., St. John, T., Paterson, S., Elison, J. T., et al. (2015). Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 7, 24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2009). A two-factor model of temperament. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(6), 565–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, J. E., Landa, R., Bhat, A., & Bauman, M. (2012). Head lag in infants at risk for autism: A preliminary study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(5), 577–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Smith, I. M., Brian, J., Roberts, W., et al. (2009). Temperament and its relationship to autistic symptoms in a high-risk infant sib cohort. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(1), 59–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garon, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Smith, I. M., Brian, J., Roncadin, C., et al. (2016). Temperament and its association with autism symptoms in a high-risk population. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(4), 757–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gartstein, M., & Rothbart, M. (2003). Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 64–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, S., et al. (1987). Roundtable: What is temperament? Four approaches. Child Development, 58(2), 505–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingersoll, B., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2011). The relationship between the broader autism phenotype, child severity, and stress and depression in parents of children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 337–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Components of visual orienting in early infancy: Contingency learning, anticipatory looking, and disengaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 335–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, R., & Bryson, S. E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(6), 1115–1122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecouteur, A., Rutter, M., & Lord, C. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview: Revised. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S., Odom, S. L., & Loftin, R. (2007). Social engagement with peers and stereotypical behavior of children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9, 67–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Losh, M., & Piven, J. (2007). Social cognition and the broad autism phenotype: Identifying genetically meaningful phenotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(1), 105–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macari, S. L., Koller, J., Campbell, D. J., & Chawarska, K. (2017). Temperamental markers in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12710.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., & Palacio-Espasa, F. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1239–1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, L., Wetherby, A. M., & Barber, A. (2008). Repetitive and stereotyped movements in children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 826–837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, M., Bolton, P., Pickles, A., Fombonne, E., Piven, J., & Rutter, M. (2000). Personality traits of the relatives of autistic probands. Psychological Medicine, 30, 1411–1424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3–4), 395–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill, M. M., Hutman, T., et al. (2010). A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 256–266.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Steinfeld, M. B., Hill, M. M., Cook, I., Hutman, T., et al. (2009). How early do parent concerns predict later autism diagnosis? Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30(5), 367–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pandey, J. (2008). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT) sibling study: Are younger siblings representative of the general ASD population? Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 68, 5586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3), 386–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, S. P., Rothbart, M. K., & Gartstein, M. A. (2008). Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood. Infant and Child Development, 17(4), 387–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Temperament and the development of inhibited approach. Child Development, 59, 1241–1250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Temperament, development, and personality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 207–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional and personality development (5th ed., pp. 105–176). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 37–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M., Ellis, L., Rueda, M., & Posner, M. (2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71, 1113–1143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart, M. K., & Putnam, S. (2002). Temperament and socialization. In L. Pulkkinen & A. Caspi (Eds.), Paths to successful development: Personality in the life course (pp. 19–45). Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rozga, A., Hutman, T., Young, G. S., Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., Dapretto, M., et al. (2011). Behavioral profiles of affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: Contribution of measures of mother–infant interaction and nonverbal communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(3), 287–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacrey, L. A. R., Zwaigenbaum, L., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Georgiades, S., Brian, J., et al. (2017). Brief Report: Characteristics of preschool children with ASD vary by ascertainment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanson, A., Hemphill, S. A., & Smart, D. (2004). Connections between temperament and social development: A review. Social Development, 13(1), 142–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shen, M. D., Nordahl, C. W., Young, G. S., Wootton-Gorges, S. L., Lee, A., Liston, S. E., et al. (2013). Early brain enlargement and elevated extra-axial fluid in infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Brain, 136(9), 2825–2835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 2–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shiner, R. L., Buss, K. A., McClowry, S. G., Putnam, S. P., Saudino, K. J., & Zentner, M. (2012). What is temperament now? Assessing progress in temperament research on the twenty‐fifth anniversary of Goldsmith et al. (†). Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 436–444.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Oxford, England: Brunner/Mazel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, J., Botteron, K., Dager, S., Elison, J., Estes, A., Gu, H., et al. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(8), 945–953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zentner, M., & Bates, J. E. (2008). Child temperament: An integrative review of concepts, research programs, and measures. International Journal of Developmental Science, 2(1–2), 7–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. Special Issue: Autism: Modeling Human Brain Abnormalities in Developing Animal Systems, 23(2–3), 143–152.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by Grants from NIH/NIHCD (R01-HD055741, HD055741-S1) Autism Speaks, and the Simons Foundation to J. Piven and K01-MH101653 to J. Wolff. We sincerely thank our IBIS families for participating in this research. We are grateful to Robert Emerson for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

* IBIS Network: The IBIS (Infant Brain Imaging Study) Network is an NIH funded Autism Centers of Excellence project and consists of a consortium of 8 Universities in the U.S. and Canada. Clinical Sites: University of North Carolina: J. Piven (IBIS Network PI), H.C. Hazlett, C. Chappell; University of Washington: S. Dager, A. Estes, D. Shaw; Washington University: K. N. Botteron, R. C. McKinstry, J. Constantino, J. Pruett; Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia: R. T. Schultz, S. Paterson; University of Alberta: L. Zwaigenbaum; University of Minnesota: J. Elison. Data Coordinating Center: Montreal Neurological Institute: A.C. Evans, D. L. Collins, G. B. Pike, V. Fonov, P. Kostopoulos, S. Das. Image Processing Core: New York University: G. Gerig; University of North Carolina: M. Styner. Statistical Analysis Core: University of North Carolina: H. Gu.

Funding

This study was funded by National Institutes of Health (R01-HD 055741, R01-HD055741-S1, K01-MH101653) and the Simons Foundation (SFARI Grant 140209) and Autism Speaks.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Contributions

SJP, AE, LZ, RTS, JP, HCH, SRD, KB, JJW, JTE and JPn, contributed to the concept and design of the study, provided feedback, and reviewed the manuscript. SJP and JJW performed statistical analyses and interpreted the data. SJP, JJW, JTE, BWP, and JPn contributed to drafting and revising of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sarah J. Paterson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The members of the IBIS Network group are listed in the acknowledgements.

Electronic supplementary material

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Paterson, S.J., Wolff, J.J., Elison, J.T. et al. The Importance of Temperament for Understanding Early Manifestations of Autism Spectrum Disorder in High-Risk Infants. J Autism Dev Disord 49, 2849–2863 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04003-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04003-2

Keywords

Navigation