Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Speech Acts During Friends’ and Non-friends’ Spontaneous Conversations in Preschool Dyads with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder versus Typical Development

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study, we videotaped two 10-min. free-play interactions and coded speech acts (SAs) in peer talk of 51 preschoolers (21 ASD, 30 typical), interacting with friend versus non-friend partners. Groups were matched for maternal education, IQ (verbal/nonverbal), and CA. We compared SAs by group (ASD/typical), by partner’s friendship status (friend/non-friend), and by partner’s disability status. Main results yielded a higher amount and diversity of SAs in the typical than the ASD group (mainly in assertive acts, organizational devices, object-dubbing, and pretend-play); yet, those categories, among others, showed better performance with friends versus non-friends. Overall, a more nuanced perception of the pragmatic deficit in ASD should be adopted, highlighting friendship as an important context for children’s development of SAs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner review: The assessment of language pragmatics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 973–987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, C., Green, J., Gilchrist, A., & Cox, A. (2002). Conversational behaviour of children with Asperger syndrome and conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 679–690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Text revision (5th edn.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bauminger, N., Solomon, N., Aviezer, A., Heung, K., Brown, J., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). Friendship in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder: Mixed and non-mixed dyads. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1211–1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauminger-Zviely, N., & Agam-Ben-Artzi, G. (2014). Young friendship in HFASD and typical development: friend versus non- friend comparisons. Journal of Autism Development Disorder, 44, 1733–1748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauminger-Zviely, N., Karin, E., Kimhi, Y., & Agam-Ben-Artzi, G. (2014). Spontaneous peer conversation in preschoolers with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder versus typical development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 363–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bone, D., Lee, C., Chaspari, T., Black, M. P., Williams, M. E., Lee, S., Levitt, P., & Narayanan, S. (2013). Acoustic-prosodic, turn-taking, and language cues in child-psychologist interactions for varying social demand. INTERSPEECH, 2400–2404.

  • Brachfeld-Child, S., & Schiavo, R. S. (1990). Interactions of preschool and kindergarten friends and acquaintances. Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human Development, 151(1), 45–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delays. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2, 325–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dore, J. (1979). Conversational acts and the acquisition of language. In E. Ochs & B. B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 339–361). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dore, J. (1986). The development of conversational competence. In R. L. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Language competence assessment and intervention (pp. 3–60). San Diego: University of Kansas College Hill Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Understanding others, and individual differences in friendship interactions in young children. Social Development, 8(2), 201–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., Cutting, A. L., & Fisher, N. (2002). Old friends, new friends: Predictors of children’s perspectives on their friends at school. Child Development, 73, 621–636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eigsti, I. M., De Marchena, A. B., Schuh, J. M., & Kelley, E. (2011). Language acquisition in autism spectrum disorder: a developmental review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 681–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1985). The logico-affective disorder in autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Communication problems in autism (pp.<background-color:#96C864;> </background-color:#96C864;>283–310). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Howes, C. (1996). The earliest friendships. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 66–86). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C. (2003). A review of research into pretend play in autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 7(4), 379–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. D., & Schwartz, I. S. (2009). When asking questions is not enough: An observational study of social communication differences in high functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 432–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, R. (2003). Social play and autistic spectrum disorders: A perspective on theory, implications and educational approaches. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 7, 347–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., Locke, J., Gulsrud, A., & Rotheram-Fuller, E. (2011). Social networks and friendships at school: Comparing children with and without ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 533–544.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasari, C., & Ya-Chin, C. (2013). Play development in children with autism spectrum disorders: Skills, object play, and intervention. In F. R. Volkmar, S. J. Rogers, R. Paul, &amp & K. A. Pelphery (Eds.), Handbook of autism and developmental disorders, 4th edition (pp. 230–262). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kissine, M., Cano-Chervel, J., Carlier, S., De Brabanter, P., Ducenne, L., Pairon, M.C., Deconinck, N., Delvenne, V., & Leybaert, J. (2015). Children with autism understand indirect speech acts: Evidence from a semi-structured act-out task. PLoS ONE, 10(11), Article e0142191.

  • Kjellmer, L., Hedvall, Å., Fernell, E., Gillberg, C., & Norrelgen, F. (2012). Language and communication skills in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders: Contribution of cognition, severity of autism symptoms, and adaptive functioning to the variability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 172–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., & Uljarevic, M. (2011). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: A review of research in the last decade. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 562–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (1995). Peer interactions of autistic children and adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 7(4), 611–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller-Wetherby, A., & Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship relations: A meta analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 306–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paparella, T., Goods, K. S., Freeman, S., & Kasari, C. (2011). The emergence of joint attention and requesting skills in young children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 569–583.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC 2001. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumet, M. H., & Veneziano, E. (2015). Typical and atypical pragmatic functioning of ASD children and their partners: A study of oppositional episodes in everyday interactions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(1), 53–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview—Revised (ADI-R) manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryckebusch, C., & Marcos, H. (2004). Speech acts, social context and parent toddler play between the ages of 1:5 and 2:3. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 883–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), v-114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., & Herman, J. (1996). Learning how to say what one means: A longitudinal study of children’s speech act use. Social Development, 5, 56–84.

  • Stefanatos, G. A., & Baron, I. S. (2011). The ontogenesis of language impairment in autism: A neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 21, 252–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., & Caro-Martinez, L. M. (1990). Naturalistic observations of spontaneous communication in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(4), 437–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziatas, K., Durkin, K., & Pratt, C. (2003). Differences in assertive speech acts produced by children with autism, Asperger syndrome, specific language impairment, and normal development. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 73–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was partially supported by the Israel Science Foundation given to the first author. Special thanks are extended to the children who took part in this study. The authors would like to express their appreciation to Dee B. Ankonina for her editorial contribution and to Dov Har–Even for his statistical assistance.

Funding

This research was partially supported by the Israel Science Foundation (ISF/ No.502/06) to the first author, Nirit Bauminger-Zviley.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nirit Bauminger-Zviely.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendix

Appendix

Dore’s (1979) Taxonomy Subcategories: Description and Examples of Assertives, Requestives, and Responsives

Assertives

Declarations These utterances create social facts, such as:procedurals for invoking norms or rules, setting procedures, or defining conditions (“This is not the right way to build a pyramid,” “This is the sea and here are the bears”); claims for the speaker’s rights (“I’m first,” “My turn now”); the use of jokes (“We throw soup on the roof”), teasing (“You can’t catch me”), and warnings (“Pay attention,” “Be careful”).

Evaluations These utterances judge situations/conditions, such as:evaluatives that express personal judgment or attitudes (“These are ugly things,” “This is fun”); attributions that express beliefs about another’s state (“He wants to go,” “He loves her”); and explanations that express casual relations, give reasons, or make predictions (“He’s crying because he got hit,” “I did it because it was fun”).

Reports These utterances represent existing states, such as:identifications that label objects and/or events (e.g., “This is a car”); descriptions (“It fell,” “It’s the same”); sharings that describe events referring personally to the speaker, beyond the “here and now” (“My friend Orr had grape juice in his old house”); and internal reports of mental states and emotions (“I like it,” “That hurts”).

Requestives

Questions These utterances include:choice questions (Yes or no?); product WH questions (Who? Which? Where? When? What?); and process questions that seek explanation or extend description (Why? How? What for? What about? How come?).

Requests These utterances include actions (“Sit,” “Give me”); permissions (“Can I take this?”); and action suggestions (“Let’s do this”).

Responsives

Answers These utterances provide answers to choice (“Yes;” “No”), product (“He’s here”), and process (“I wanted to”) questions, as well as compliances (“Okay”) and clarifications that are not necessarily in reply to questions (“I said no,” “Really,” “Just kidding”).

Unsolicited replies These replies to non-soliciting utterances include:descriptions of qualifications (“But I can’t do it”); agreements (“Exactly”) or rejections (“Not true”); and acknowledgments (“Aha”).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bauminger-Zviely, N., Golan-Itshaky, A. & Tubul-Lavy, G. Speech Acts During Friends’ and Non-friends’ Spontaneous Conversations in Preschool Dyads with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder versus Typical Development. J Autism Dev Disord 47, 1380–1390 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3064-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3064-x

Keywords

Navigation